Gifford-Gonzalez, D. Bones are not enough: Analogues, knowledge, and interpretive strategies in zooarchaeology. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 10, 215–254 (1991).
Google Scholar
Pobiner, B. L. The zooarchaeology and paleoecology of early hominin scavenging. Evol. Anthropol. 2, 68–82 (2020).
Google Scholar
Rodriguez, A. et al. Right or left? Determining the hand holding the tool from use traces. J. Archaeol. Sci. Rep. 31, 102316 (2020).
Feix, T., Kivell, T. L., Pouydebat, E. & Dollar, A. M. Estimating thumb-index finger precision grip and manipulation potential in extant and fossil primates. J. R. Soc. Interface. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2015.0176 (2015).
Google Scholar
Bardo, A. et al. The implications of thumb movements for Neanderthal and modern human manipulation. Sci. Rep. 10, 19323 (2020).
Google Scholar
Stout, D., Semaw, S., Rogers, M. J. & Cauche, D. Technological variation in the earliest Oldowan from Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. J. Hum. Evol. 58, 474–491 (2010).
Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Premo, L. S., Braun, D. R. & McPherron, S. P. Resetting the null hypothesis: Early stone tools and cultural transmission. Curr. Anthrop. 58, 652–672 (2017).
Google Scholar
Tennie, C. The zone of latent solution (ZLS) account remains the most parsimonious explanation for early stone tools. Curr. Anthrop. 60, 331–332 (2019).
Tennie, C., Braun, D. R., Premo, L. S. & McPherron, S. P. The Island Test for Cumulative Culture in Paleolithic Cultures. In The Nature of Culture. Series: Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology (eds Haidle, M. N. et al.) (Springer, 2016).
Perreault, C. The Quality of the Archaeological Record (University of Chicago Press, 2019).
Google Scholar
Proffitt, T. et al. Wild monkeys flake stone tools. Nature 539, 85–88 (2016).
Google Scholar
Carvalho, S., Cunha, E., Sousa, C. & Matsuzawa, T. Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking. J. Hum. Evol. 55, 148–163 (2008).
Google Scholar
Westergaard, G. C. & Suomi, S. J. The stone tools of capuchins (Cebus apella). Int. J. Primatol. 16, 1017–1024 (1995).
Google Scholar
De la Torre, I. & Mora, R. Technological Strategies in the Lower Pleistocene at Olduvai Beds I and II (Service de Prehistoire, Universite de Liege, 2005).
M. D. O. M. Í. Dominguez-Rodrigo, 3.3-Million-Year-Old Stone Tools and Butchery Traces? More Evidence Needed. PaleoAnthropology. 9 (2016).
Harmand, S. et al. 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature 521, 310–315 (2015).
Google Scholar
Andrefsky, W. Lithics: Macroscopic Approaches to Analysis (Cambridge University Press, 2005).
Google Scholar
Malaivijitnond, S. et al. Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Am. J. Primatol. 69, 227–233 (2007).
Google Scholar
Luncz, L. V. et al. Resource depletion through primate stone technology. eLife 6, e23647 (2017).
Google Scholar
Leca, J. B., Gunst, N. & Huffman, M. Complexity in object manipulation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): A cross-sectional analysis of manual coordination in stone handling patterns. J. Comp. Psychol. 125, 61 (2011).
Google Scholar
Toth, N., Schick, K. D., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Sevcik, R. A. & Rumbaugh, D. M. Pan the tool-maker: Investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo (Pan paniscus). J. Archaeol. Sci. 20, 81–91 (1993).
Google Scholar
Wright, R. V. S. Imitative learning of a flaked stone technology-The case of an orangutan. Mankind 8, 296–306 (2009).
Bandini, E. et al. Naïve, unenculturated chimpanzees fail to make and use flaked stone tools. Open Res. Eur. https://doi.org/10.12688/openreseurope.13186.2 (2021).
Google Scholar
J. Henrich, C. Tennie, in Chimpanzees and Human Evolution, M. Muller, R. Wrangham, D. Pilbeam, Eds. (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, (2017), 645–702.
Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M. & Fishlock, V. First observation of tool use in wild gorillas. PLoS Biol. 3, 2041–2043 (2005).
Google Scholar
Wittiger, L., Society, W. C., River, C. & Project, G. Tool use during display behavior in wild cross river gorillas. Am. J. Primat. 5, 1–5 (2007).
Kinani, J. F. & Zimmerman, D. Tool use for food acquisition in a wild mountain gorilla (Gorilla beringei beringei). Am. J. Primat. 77, 353–357 (2015).
Google Scholar
Grueter, C. C., Robbins, M. M., Ndagijimana, F. & Stoinski, T. S. Possible tool use in a mountain gorilla. Behav. Processes. 100, 160–162 (2013).
Google Scholar
Parker, S. T., Kerr, M., Markowitz, H. & Gould, J. A survey of tool use in zoo gorillas. In The Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans: Comparative Perspectives (eds Parker, S. T. et al.) (Cambridge University Press, 1999).
Google Scholar
Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R. & Beck, B. B. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals (Johns Hopkins University Press, 2011).
Pouydebat, E., Berge, C., Gorce, P. & Coppens, Y. Use and manufacture of tools to extract food by captive Gorilla gorilla gorilla: Experimental approach. Folia Primat. 76, 180–183. https://doi.org/10.1159/000084381 (2005).
Google Scholar
Haslam, M. ‘Captivity bias’ in animal tool use and its implications for the evolution of hominin technology. PTRBAE 368, 20120421 (2013).
Google Scholar
Van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O. & Merrill, M. Y. The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 719–741 (1999).
Google Scholar
Pouydebat, E., Gorce, P., Coppens, Y. & Bels, V. Biomechanical study of grasping according to the volume of the object: Human versus non-human primates. J. Biomech. 42, 266–272 (2009).
Google Scholar
Pouydebat, E., Laurin, M., Gorce, P. & Bels, V. Evolution of grasping among anthropoids. J. Evol. Bio. 21, 1732–1743 (2008).
Google Scholar
Bardo, A., Cornette, R., Borel, A. & Pouydebat, E. Manual function and performance in humans, gorillas and orangutans during the same tool use task. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.2332 (2017).
Google Scholar
A. Bardo, A. Borel, H. Meunier, J. P. Guéry, E. Pouydebat, Manual abilities in great apes during a tool use task. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. doi: 10.1002 (2016).
W. C. McGrew, Why is ape tool use so confusing. Comparative socioecology: the behavioural ecology of humans and other mammals. 457–472 (1989).
Cipolletta, C. et al. Termite feeding by Gorilla gorilla gorilla at Bai Hokou, Central African Republic. Int. J. Primatol. 28, 457–476 (2007).
Google Scholar
Salmi, R., Rahman, U. & Doran-Sheehy, D. M. Hand preference for a novel bimanual coordinated task during termite feeding in wild western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Int. J. Primatol. 37, 200–212 (2016).
Google Scholar
Masi, S. et al. The influence of seasonal frugivory on nutrient and energy intake in wild western gorillas. PLoS ONE 10, e0129254 (2015).
Google Scholar
Redford, K. H. & Dorea, J. G. The nutritional value of invertebrates with emphasis on ants and termites as food for mammals. J. Zool. 203, 385–395 (1984).
Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. The ‘other faunivory’revisited: Insectivory in human and non-human primates and the evolution of human diet. J. Hum. Evol. 71, 4–11 (2014).
Google Scholar
Tennie, C., O’Malley, R. C. & Gilby, I. C. Why do chimpanzees hunt? Considering the benefits and costs of acquiring and consuming vertebrate versus invertebrate prey. J. Hum. Evol. 71, 38–45 (2014).
Google Scholar
McBrearty, S. Consider the humble termite: Termites as agents of post-depositional disturbance at African archaeological sites. J. Archaeol. Sci. 17, 111–143 (1990).
Google Scholar
Okwakol, M. J. N. Effects of Cubitermes testaceus (Williams) on some physical and chemical properties of soil in a grassland area of Uganda. Afr. J. Ecol. 25, 147–153 (1987).
Google Scholar
Altmann, J. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behavior 49, 227–267 (1974).
Google Scholar
Robira, B. et al. Handedness in gestural and manipulative actions in male hunter-gatherer Aka pygmies from Central African Republic. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 166(481–491), 19 (2018).
Meguerditchian, A., Calcutt, S. E., Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R. & Hopkins, W. D. Brief communication: Captive gorillas are right-handed for bimanual feeding. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 141, 638–645 (2010).
Google Scholar
Dapena, J. E. S. Ú. S., William, J., Anderst, N. P. & Toth, The biomechanics of the arm swing in Oldowan stone flaking. In The Oldowan: Case Studies into the Earliest Stone Age (No. 1). Gosport (eds Toth, N. P. & Schick, K. D.) (Stone Age Institute Press, 2006).
Nowell, A. A. & Fletcher, A. W. The development of feeding behaviour in wild western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Behaviour 145, 171–193 (2008).
Google Scholar
Pouydebat, E., Gorce, P., Coppens, Y. & Bels, V. Substrate optimization in nuts cracking by capuchin monkeys. Am. J. Primatol. 68, 1017–1024 (2006).
Google Scholar
Boinski, S., Quatrone, R. P. & Swarttz, H. Substrate and tool use by brown capuchins in Suriname: Ecological contexts and cognitive bases. Am. Anthropol. 102, 741–761 (2000).
Google Scholar
Panger, M. A. Object-use in free-ranging white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Costa Rica. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 106, 311–321 (1998).
Google Scholar
Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R. Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in cebus monkeys and great apes. J. Hum. Evol. 6, 623–641 (1977).
Google Scholar
Heldstab, S. A., Isler, K., Schuppli, C. & van Schaik, C. P. When ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny: Fixed neurodevelopmental sequence of manipulative skills among primates. Sci. Adv. 6, eabb4685 (2020).
Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H. Some aspects of intraspecific variation in feeding and ranging behaviour in primates. In Primate Ecology Studies of Feeding And Ranging Behavior in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes (ed. Clutton-Brock, T. H.) (Academic Press, 1977).
Key, C. & Ross, C. Sex differences in energy expenditure in non-human primates. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. 266, 2479–2485 (1999).
Google Scholar
Lockman, J. J. A perception–action perspective on tool use development. Child Dev. 71, 137–144 (2000).
Google Scholar
Masi, S. et al. Unusual feeding behavior in wild great apes, a window to understand origins of self-medication in humans: Role of sociality and physiology on learning process. Physiol. Behav. 105, 337–349 (2012).
Google Scholar
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. The manifold use of pounding stone tools by wild capuchin monkeys of Serra da Capivara National Park, Brazil. Behaviour 153, 421–442 (2016).
Google Scholar
Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E. B. Stone throwing as a sexual display in wild female bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus. PLoS ONE 8, e79535 (2013).
Google Scholar
Mannu, M. & Ottoni, E. B. The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: Tool making, associative use, and secondary tools. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 242–251 (2009).
Google Scholar
Gumert, M. D., Kluck, M. & Malaivijitnond, S. Thephysical characteristics and usage patterns of stoneaxe and pounding hammers used by long-tailedmacaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 594–608. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20694 (2009).
Google Scholar
Marzke, M. W. Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 102, 91–110 (1997).
Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. Chimpanzee Intelligence in Nature and in Captivity Isomorphism of Symbol Use and Tool Use (Cambridge University Press, 1996).
Google Scholar
Westergaard, G. C. & Suomi, S. J. A simple stone-tool technology in monkeys. J. Hum. Evol. 27, 399–404 (1994).
Google Scholar
Liu, Q. et al. Kinematics and energetics of nut-cracking in wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in Piauí, Brazil. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 138, 210–220 (2009).
Google Scholar
Günther, M. M. & Boesch, C. Energetic Cost of Nut-cracking Behaviour in Wild Chimpanzees. In Hands of Primates 109–129 (Springer, 1993).
Roach, N. T., Venkadesan, M., Rainbow, M. J. & Lieberman, D. E. Elastic energy storage in the shoulder and the evolution of high-speed throwing in Homo. Nature 498, 483–486 (2013).
Google Scholar
Young, N. M., Capellini, T. D., Roach, N. T. & Alemseged, Z. Fossil hominin shoulders support an African ape-like last common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees. PNAS 112, 11829–11834 (2015).
Google Scholar
Doran-Sheehy, D., Mongo, P., Lodwick, J. & Conklin-Brittain, N. L. Male and female western gorilla diet: Preferred foods, use of fallback resources, and implications for ape versus old world monkey foraging strategies. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 140, 727–738 (2009).
Google Scholar
Breuer, T., Hockemba, M. B. N., Olejniczak, C., Parnell, R. J. & Stokes, E. J. Physical maturation, life-history classes and age estimates of free-ranging western gorillas – Insights from Mbeli Bai, Republic of Congo. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 106–119 (2009).
Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D. et al. The use of bouts and frequencies in the evaluation of hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): An empirical study comparing two different indices of laterality. J. Comp. Psychol. 115, 294–299 (2001).
Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Byrne, J. M. Manual dexterity in the gorilla: bimanual and digit role differentiation in a natural task. Anim. Cogn. 4, 347–361 (2001).
Google Scholar
Source: Ecology - nature.com