in

Contact calls in woodpeckers are individually distinctive, show significant sex differences and enable mate recognition

  • 1.

    Catchpole, C. K. Variation in the song of the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus in relation to mate attraction and territorial defence. Anim. Behav. 31, 1217–1225 (1983).

    Google Scholar 

  • 2.

    Andersson, M. Sexual selection (University Press, 1994).

    Google Scholar 

  • 3.

    Searcy, W. A. & Yasukawa, K. Song and female choice. In Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds (eds Kroodsma, D. E. & Miller, E. H.) 454–473 (Cornell University Press, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • 4.

    O’Loghlen, A. L. & Beecher, M. D. Mate, neighbour and stranger song: A female song sparrow perspective. Anim. Behav. 58, 13–20 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  • 5.

    Gentner, T. Q. & Hulse, S. H. Female European starling preference and choice for variation in conspecific male song. Anim. Behav. 59, 443–458 (2000).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 6.

    Molles, L. E. & Vehrencamp, S. L. Neighbour recognition by resident males in the banded wren Thryothorus pleurostictus a tropical songbird with high song type sharing. Anim. Behav. 61, 119–127 (2001).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 7.

    Ballentine, B., Hyman, J. & Nowicki, S. Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: An experimental test. Behav. Ecol. 15, 163–168 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • 8.

    Forstmeier, W., Kempenaers, B., Meyer, A. & Leisler, B. A novel song parameter correlates with extra-pair paternity and reflects male longevity. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 1479–1485 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • 9.

    de Kort, S. R., Eldermire, E. R. B., Valderrama, S., Botero, C. A. & Vehrencamp, S. L. Trill consistency is an age-related assessment signal in banded wrens. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 276, 2315–2321 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • 10.

    Węgrzyn, E., Leniowski, K. & Osiejuk, T. Whistle duration and consistency reflect philopatry and harem size in great reed warblers. Anim. Behav. 79, 1363–1372 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • 11.

    Węgrzyn, E., Leniowski, K. & Osiejuk, T. Introduce yourself at the beginning – Possibile identification function of the initial part of the song in the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus. Ornis Fennica 86, 61–70 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • 12.

    Węgrzyn, E. & Leniowski, K. Middle Spotted Woodpecker territory owners distinguish between stranger and familiar foaters based on their vocal characteristics. Eur. Zool. J. 87, 58–72 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  • 13.

    Podos, J. Motor constraints on vocal development in a songbird. Anim. Behav. 51, 1061–1070 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • 14.

    Podos, J., Southall, J. A. & Rossi-Santos, M. R. Vocal mechanics in Darwin’s finches: Correlation of beak gape and song frequency. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 607–619 (2004).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 15.

    Nelson, B. S., Deckers, G. J. L. & Suthers, R. A. Vocal tract filtering and sound radiation in a songbird. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 297–308 (2005).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 16.

    Falls, J. B. Individual recognition by sounds in birds. In Acoustic communication in birds Vol. 2 (eds Kroodsma, D. E. & Miller, E. H.) 237–278 (Academic Press, 1982).

    Google Scholar 

  • 17.

    Wiley, R. H., Hatchwell, B. J. & Davies, N. B. Recognition of individual males songs by female dunnocks: A mechanism increasing the number of copulatory partners and reproductive success. Ethology 88, 145–153 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  • 18.

    Lind, H., Dabelsteen, T. & McGregor, P. K. Female great tits can identify mates by song. Anim. Behav. 52, 667–671 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • 19.

    Aubin, T., Jouventin, P. & Hildebrand, C. Penguins use the two-voice system to recognize each other. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 1081–1087 (2000).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • 20.

    Charrier, I., Jouventin, P., Mathevon, N. & Aubin, T. Individual identity coding depends on call type in the South Polar Skua Catharacta maccormicki. Polar Biol. 24, 378–382 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 21.

    Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., Horning, C. L. & Willis, M. S. Strong neighbor– stranger discrimination in song sparrows. Condor 92, 1051–1056 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  • 22.

    Stoddard, P. K., Beecher, M. D., Horning, C. L. & Campbell, S. E. Recognition of individual neighbors by song in the song sparrow, a species with song repertoires. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29, 211–215 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  • 23.

    Godard, R. Long–term memory of individual neighbours in a migratory songbird. Nature 350, 228–229 (1991).

    ADS 

    Google Scholar 

  • 24.

    Stoddard, P. K. Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines. In Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds (eds Kroodsma, D. E. & Miller, E. H.) 56–374 (Cornell University Press, 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • 25.

    Hyman, J. Seasonal variation in response to neighbors and strangers by a territorial songbird. Ethology 111, 951–961 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • 26.

    Mackin, W. A. Neighbor–stranger discrimination in Audubon’s Shearwater Puffinus l. lherminieri explained by a “real enemy” effect. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59(2), 326–332 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  • 27.

    Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., Jouventin, P. & Aubin, T. Acoustic communication in a Black-headed Gull colony: How do chicks identify their parents?. Ethology 107, 961–974 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 28.

    Lengagne, T., Lauga, J. & Aubin, T. Intra–syllabic acoustic signatures used by the King Penguin in parent–chick recognition: An experimental approach. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 663–672 (2001).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 29.

    Jouventin, P. & Aubin, T. Acoustic systems are adapted to breeding ecologies: Individual recognition in nesting penguins. Anim. Behav. 64, 747–757 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • 30.

    Cucco, M. & Malacarne, G. Is the song of black restart males an honest signal of status?. Condor 101, 689–694 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  • 31.

    Christie, P. J., Mennill, D. J. & Ratcliffe, L. M. Chickadee song structure is individually distinctive over long broadcast distances. Behaviour 141, 101–124 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • 32.

    Sherman, P. W., Reeve, H. K. & Pfennig D. W. Recognition systems. In: Krebs JR,DaviesNB, editors. Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific. pp. 69–96 (1997).

  • 33.

    Kilham, L. Behavior and methods of communication of Pileated woodpeckers. Condor 61, 377–387 (1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • 34.

    Lawrence, L. & de Kort, S. R. A comparative life–history study of four species of woodpeckers. Ornithol. Monogr. 5, 1–155 (1967).

    Google Scholar 

  • 35.

    Winkler, H. & Short, L. A comparative analysis of acoustical signals in Pied woodpeckers (Aves, Picoides). Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 160, 1–110 (1978).

    Google Scholar 

  • 36.

    Crusoe, D. A. Acoustic behavior and its role in the social relations of the red-headed wood-pecker: Picidae, Melanerpes erythrocephalus. Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Chicago Circle (1980).

  • 37.

    Pardo, M. A. et al. Wild acorn woodpeckers recognize associations between individuals in other groups. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20181017 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 38.

    Leniowski, K. & Węgrzyn, E. The carotenoid-based red cap of the middle spotted woodpecker Dendrocopos medius reflects individual quality and territory size. Ibis 155(4), 804–813 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • 39.

    Podos, J., Lahti, D. C. & Moseley, D. L. Vocal performance and sensorimotor learning in songbirds. Adv. Study Behav. 40, 159–195 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • 40.

    Tremain, S. B., Swiston, K. A. & Mennill, D. J. Seasonal variation in acoustic signals of Pileated Woodpeckers. Wilson J. Ornithol. 120(3), 499–504 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • 41.

    Kilham, L. Reproductive behavior of red–bellied woodpeckers. Wilson Bull. 73, 237–254 (1961).

    Google Scholar 

  • 42.

    Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. Bird Song. Biological themes and variation 2nd edn. (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • 43.

    Falls, J. B. & McNicholl, M. K. Neighbor–stranger discrimination by song in male blue grouse. Can. J. Zool. 57(2), 457–462 (1979).

    Google Scholar 

  • 44.

    Galeotti, P. & Pavan, G. Individual recognition of male tawny owls Strix aluco using spectrograms of their territorial calls. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 3(2), 113–126 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  • 45.

    Prum, R. O. Sexual selection and the evolution of mechanical sound production in manakins Aves: Pipridae. Anim. Behav. 55(4), 977–994 (1998).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 46.

    Rebbeck, M., Corrick, R., Eaglestone, B. & Stainton, C. Recognition of individual European Nightjars Caprimulgus europaeus from their song. Ibis 143, 468–475 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 47.

    Dodenhoff, D. J. An analysis of acoustic communication within the social system of downy woodpeckers Picoides pubescens. Doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University. (2002).

  • 48.

    Budka, M., Deoniziak, K., Tumiel, T. & Białas, J. T. Vocal individuality in drumming in great spotted woodpecker—A biological perspective and implications for conservation. PLoS ONE 13(2), e0191716 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 49.

    Ydenberg, R. C., Giraldeau, L. A. & Falls, J. B. Neighbours, strangers, and the asymmetric war of attrition. Anim. Behav. 36(2), 343–347 (1988).

    Google Scholar 

  • 50.

    Delport, W., Kemp, A. C. & Ferguson, W. H. Vocal identification of individual African Wood Owls Strix woodfordii: a technique to monitor long-term adult turnover and residency. Ibis 144, 30–39 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • 51.

    Peake, T. M. et al. Individuality in Corncrake Crex crex vocalisations. Ibis 140, 120–217 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  • 52.

    Hoodless, A. N., Inglis, J. G., Doucet, J.-P. & Aebischer, N. J. Vocal individuality in the roding calls of Woodcock Scolopax rusticola and their use to validate a survey method. Ibis 150, 80–89 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • 53.

    Grava, T., Mathevon, N., Place, E. & Balluet, P. Individual acoustic monitoring of the European Eagle Owl Bubo bubo. Ibis 150, 279–287 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • 54.

    Odom, K. J., Slaght, J. C. & Gutierrez, R. J. Distinctiveness in the territorial calls of Great horned owls within and among years. J. Raptor Res. 47, 21–30 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • 55.

    Aubin, T., Mathevon, N., Staszewski, V. & Boulinier, T. Acoustic communication in the Kittiwake Rissa tridactyla: potential cues for sexual and individual signatures in ling calls. Polar Biol. 30, 1027–1033 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  • 56.

    Bretagnolle, V. & Laquette, B. Structural variation in the call of the Cory’s Shearwater (Colonectris diodemea, Aves, Procellaridae). Ethology 85, 313–323 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  • 57.

    de Broke, M. L. Sexual differences in the voice and individual vocal recognition in the Manx Shearwater (Puffinus Puffinus). Anim. Behav. 26, 622–629 (1978).

    Google Scholar 

  • 58.

    Dreiss, A. N., Ruppli, C. A. & Roulin, A. Individual vocal signatures in barn owl nestling: does individual recognition have an adaptive role in sibling vocal competition?. J. Evol. Biol. 27, 63–75 (2014).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 59.

    Volodin, I. A., Volodina, E. V., Klenova, A. V. & Filatova, O. A. Individual and sexual differences in the calls of the monomorphic White-faced Whistling Duck Dendrocygna viduata. Acta Ornithol. 40, 43–52 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  • 60.

    Bragina, E. & Beme, J. sexual and individual features in the long range and short range calls of the White-naped crane. Condor 115, 501–507 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • 61.

    Terry, A. M. R., Peake, T. M. & McGregor, P. K. The role of vocal individuality in conservation. Front. Zool. 2, 10 (2005).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 62.

    Pasinelli, G. Oaks (Quercus sp.) and only oaks? Relations between habitat structure and home range size of the middle spotted woodpecker (Dendrocopos medius). Biol. Conserv. 93(2), 227–235 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • 63.

    Pasinelli, G. Dendrocopos medius middle spotted woodpecker. BWP Update 5(1), 49–99 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  • 64.

    Michalek, K. G. & Winkler, H. Parental care and parentage in monogamous great spotted woodpeckers Picoides major and middle spotted woodpeckers Picoides medius. Behaviour 138(10), 1259–1285 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 65.

    Pasinelli, G., Hegelbach, J. & Reyer, H.-U. Spacing behavior of the Middle Spotted Woodpecker in central Europe. J. Wildl. Manag. 65, 432–441 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 66.

    Pasinelli, G. Breeding performance of the middle spotted woodpecker Dendrocopos medius in relation to weather and territory quality. Ardea 89, 353–361 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 67.

    Kosiński, Z. & Winiecki, A. Ocena liczebności dzięcioła średniego Dendrocopos medius – Porównanie metody kartograficznej z użyciem stymulacji magnetofonowej z metodą wyszukiwania gniazd. Notatki Ornitologiczne 44, 43–55 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  • 68.

    Specht, R. Avisoft-SASLab Pro: sound analysis and synthesis laboratory (Avisoft Bioacoustics, 2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • 69.

    Mundry, R. & Sommer, C. Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Anim. Behav. 74, 965–976 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  • 70.

    Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. Using multivariate statistics 4th edn. (Allyn and Bacon, 2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • 71.

    Leniowski, K. Signaling quality in the Middle Spotted Woodpecker Dendrocopos medius: home ranges, colour ornaments and calls. (PhD thesis) Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland (2011).


  • Source: Ecology - nature.com

    Nanograins make for a seismic shift

    Hard times tear coupled seabirds apart