in

Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated

[adace-ad id="91168"]
  • 1.

    Mesoudi, A. & Thornton, A. What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20180712 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 2.

    Schofield, D. P., McGrew, W. C., Takahashi, A. & Hirata, S. Cumulative culture in nonhumans: overlooked findings from Japanese monkeys? Primates 59, 113–122 (2018).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 3.

    Boesch, C. & Tomasello, M. Chimpanzee and human cultures. Curr. Anthropol. 39, 591–614 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  • 4.

    Tennie, C., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 2405–2415 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • 5.

    Dean, L. G., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E. & Kendal, R. L. Human cumulative culture: a comparative perspective. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 89, 284–301 (2014).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 6.

    Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S. & Hopper, L. M. Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 2417–2428 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • 7.

    Tennie, C., Bandini, E., van Schaik, C. P. & Hopper, L. M. The zone of latent solutions and its relevance to understanding ape cultures. Biol. Philos. 35, 55 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 8.

    Sasaki, T. & Biro, D. Cumulative culture can emerge from collective intelligence in animal groups. Nat. Commun. 8, 15049 (2017).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 9.

    Musgrave, S. et al. Teaching varies with task complexity in wild chimpanzees. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 969–976 (2020).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 10.

    Boesch, C. et al. Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 910–916 (2020).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 11.

    Whiten, A. The burgeoning reach of animal culture. Science 372, eabe6514 (2021).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 12.

    Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685 (1999).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • 13.

    Sanz, C. & Morgan, D. Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. J. Hum. Evol. 52, 420–433 (2007).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 14.

    Whiten, A. & van Schaik, C. P. The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 603–620 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  • 15.

    McGrew, W. C. The Cultured Chimpanzee: Reflections on Cultural Primatology (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2004).

  • 16.

    McGrew, W. C. Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1992).

  • 17.

    Sanz, C., Schöning, C. & Morgan, D. Chimpanzees prey on army ants with specialized tool set. Am. J. Primatol. 72, 17–24 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 18.

    Reindl, E., Apperly, I. A., Beck, S. R. & Tennie, C. Young children copy cumulative technological design in the absence of action information. Sci. Rep. 7, 1788 (2017).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 19.

    Neadle, D., Allritz, M. & Tennie, C. Food cleaning in gorillas: social learning is a possibility but not a necessity. PLoS ONE 12, e0188866 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 20.

    Tennie, C., Hopper, L. M. & van Schaik, C. P. in Chimpazees in Context (eds Hopper, L. M. & Ross, S. R.) 428–453 (Univ. of Chicago Press, 2020).

  • 21.

    Bandini, E. & Tennie, C. Spontaneous reoccurrence of “scooping”, a wild tool-use behaviour, in naive chimpanzees. PeerJ 5, e3814 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 22.

    Bandini, E. & Tennie, C. Individual acquisition of “stick pounding” behavior by naive chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 81, e22987 (2019).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 23.

    Menzel, C., Fowler, A., Tennie, C. & Call, J. Leaf surface roughness elicits leaf swallowing behavior in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (P. paniscus), but not in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) or orangutans (Pongo abelii). Int. J. Primatol. 34, 533–553 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • 24.

    Tonooka, R., Tomonaga, M. & Matsuzawa, T. Acquisition and transmission of tool making and use for drinking juice in a group of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Jpn. Psychol. Res. 39, 253–265 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  • 25.

    Celli, M. L., Hirata, S. & Tomonaga, M. Socioecological influences on tool use in captive chimpanzees. Int. J. Primatol. 25, 1267–1281 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • 26.

    Bernstein-Kurtycz, L. M., Hopper, L. M., Ross, S. R. & Tennie, C. Zoo-housed chimpanzees can spontaneously use tool sets but perseverate on previously successful tool-use methods. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 7, 288–309 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  • 27.

    Boesch, C. in Evolution of Social Behaviour Patterns in Primates and Man (eds Runciman, W. G. et al.) 251–268 (Oxford Univ. Press, 1996).

  • 28.

    Neadle, D., Bandini, E. & Tennie, C. Testing the individual and social learning abilities of task-naïve captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes sp.) in a nut-cracking task. PeerJ 8, e8734 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 29.

    Biro, D. et al. Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim. Cogn. 6, 213–223 (2003).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 30.

    Boesch, C., Bombjaková, D., Meier, A. & Mundry, R. Learning curves and teaching when acquiring nut-cracking in humans and chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 9, 1515 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 31.

    Matsuzawa, T. in Chimpanzee Cultures (eds Wrangham, R. W. et al.) 351–370 (Harvard Univ. Press, 1994).

  • 32.

    Matsuzawa, T. in Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology 1–42 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2021).

  • 33.

    Boesch, C., Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B. & Joulian, F. Is nut cracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour? J. Hum. Evol. 26, 325–338 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  • 34.

    Carvalho, S., Matsuzawa, T. & McGrew, W. C. in Tool Use in Animals: Cognition and Ecology (eds Sanz, C. et al.) 225–241 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2013).

  • 35.

    Morgan, B. & Abwe, E. Chimpanzees use stone hammers in Cameroon. Curr. Biol. 16, 632–633 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • 36.

    Sumita, K., Kitahara-Frisch, J. & Norikoshi, K. The aquisition of stone-tool use in captive chimpanzees. Primates 26, 168–181 (1985).

    Google Scholar 

  • 37.

    Marshall-Pescini, S. & Whiten, A. Social learning of nut-cracking behaviour in East African sanctuary-living chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 186–194 (2008).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 38.

    Inoue-Nakamura, N. & Matsuzawa, T. Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 111, 159–173 (1997).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 39.

    van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O. & Merrill, M. The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 719–741 (1999).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 40.

    Humle, T. & Matsuzawa, T. Oil palm use by adjacent communities of chimpanzees at Bossou and Nimba Mountains, West Africa. Int. J. Primatol. 25, 551–581 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • 41.

    Koops, K., McGrew, W. C. & Matsuzawa, T. Ecology of culture: do environmental factors influence foraging tool use in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus)? Anim. Behav. 85, 175–185 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • 42.

    Koops, K., Visalberghi, E. & van Schaik, C. P. The ecology of primate material culture. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140508 (2014).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 43.

    Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T. & Sugiyama, Y. The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. & Yamagiwa, J.) (Springer, 2011).

  • 44.

    Matsuzawa, T. et al. in Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior (ed. Matsuzawa, T.) 557–574 (Springer, 2001).

  • 45.

    Bandini, E., Motes-Rodrigo, A., Steele, M. P., Rutz, C. & Tennie, C. Examining the mechanisms underlying the acquisition of animal tool behaviour. Biol. Lett. 16, 20200122 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 46.

    Koops, K., McGrew, W. C., Matsuzawa, T. & Knapp, L. A. Terrestrial nest-building by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for the tree-to-ground sleep transition in early hominins. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 148, 351–361 (2012).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 47.

    Schuppli, C. et al. The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Sci. Rep. 7, 15464 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 48.

    Roberts, G. Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. Anim. Behav. 51, 1077–1086 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • 49.

    Visalberghi, E. & Addessi, E. Seeing group members eating a familiar food enhances the acceptance of novel foods in capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 60, 69–76 (2000).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 50.

    Kalan, A. K. et al. Novelty response of wild African apes to camera traps. Curr. Biol. 29, 1211–1217 (2019).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 51.

    Gruber, T., Zuberbühler, K. & Neumann, C. Travel fosters tool use in wild chimpanzees. eLife 5, e16371 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 52.

    Grund, C., Neumann, C., Zuberbühler, K. & Gruber, T. Necessity creates opportunities for chimpanzee tool use. Behav. Ecol. 30, 1136–1144 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  • 53.

    Kühl, H. S. et al. Human impact erodes chimpanzee behavioral diversity. Science 363, 1453–1455 (2019).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 54.

    Wrangham, R. W. Chimpanzees: the culture-zone concept becomes untidy. Curr. Biol. 16, R634–R635 (2006).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 55.

    McGrew, W. C., Ham, R. M., White, L. J. T., Tutin, C. E. G. & Fernandez, M. Why don’t chimpanzees in Gabon crack nuts? Int. J. Primatol. 18, 353–374 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  • 56.

    Whiten, A. Experimental studies illuminate the cultural transmission of percussive technologies in Homo and Pan. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 370, 20140359 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • 57.

    Hirata, S., Morimura, N. & Houki, C. How to crack nuts: acquisition process in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) observing a model. Anim. Cogn. 12, 87–101 (2009).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 58.

    Hayashi, M., Mizuno, Y. & Matsuzawa, T. How does stone-tool use emerge? Introduction of stones and nuts to naïve chimpanzees in captivity. Primates 46, 91–102 (2005).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 59.

    Funk, M. Werkzeuggebrauch Beim öffnen von Niessen: Unterschiedliche Bewaltigungen des Problems bei Schimpansen und Orang-Utans (Univ. of Zurich, 1985).

  • 60.

    Visalberghi, E. Acquisition of nut‐cracking behavior by two capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Folia Primatol. 49, 168–181 (1987).

    Google Scholar 

  • 61.

    Resende, B. D., Ottoni, E. B. & Fragaszy, D. M. Ontogeny of manipulative behavior and nut-cracking in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): a perception–action perspective. Dev. Sci. 11, 828–840 (2008).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 62.

    Bandini, E., Grossmann, J., Funk, M., Albiach-Serrano, A. & Tennie, C. Naïve orangutans (Pongo abelii and Pongo pygmaeus) individually acquire nut-cracking using hammer tools. Am. J. Primatol. 83, e23304 (2021).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 63.

    Damerius, L. A. et al. Orientation toward humans predicts cognitive performance in orang-utans. Sci. Rep. 7, 40052 (2017).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 64.

    Reindl, E., Beck, S. R., Apperly, A. & Tennie, C. Young children spontaneously invent wild great apes’ tool-use behaviours. Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20152402 (2016).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 65.

    Neldner, K. et al. A cross-cultural investigation of young children’s spontaneous invention of tool use behaviours. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 192240 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 66.

    Lucas, A. J. et al. The value of teaching increases with tool complexity in cumulative cultural evolution. Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20201885 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 67.

    Boesch, C. Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Int. J. Primatol. 41, 530–532 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  • 68.

    Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R. & Sanz, C. Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 6, 34783 (2016).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • 69.

    Koops, K. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 277–287 (Springer, 2011).

  • 70.

    van Schaik, C. P. & Pfannes, K. R. in Seasonality in Primates: Studies of Living and Extinct Human and Non-human Primates (eds Brockman, D. K. & van Schaik, C. P.) 23–54 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2005).

  • 71.

    Matsuzawa, T. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 157–164 (Springer, 2011).

  • 72.

    Carvalho, S., Cunha, E., Sousa, C. & Matsuzawa, T. Chaînes opératoires and resource exploitation strategies in chimpanzee nut-cracking (Pan troglodytes). J. Hum. Evol. 55, 148–163 (2008).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 73.

    Sanz, C., Morgan, D. & Gulick, S. New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo basin. Am. Nat. 164, 67–581 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • 74.

    Hockings, K. J., Anderson, J. R. & Matsuzawa, T. Flexible feeding on cultivated underground storage organs by rainforest-dwelling chimpanzees at Bossou, West Africa. J. Hum. Evol. 58, 227–233 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 75.

    Takemoto, H. Seasonal change in terrestriality of chimpanzees in relation to microclimate in the tropical forest. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 124, 81–92 (2004).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 76.

    van Leeuwen, K. L., Matsuzawa, T., Sterck, E. H. M. & Koops, K. How to measure chimpanzee party size? A methodological comparison. Primates 61, 201–212 (2020).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • 77.

    Field, A. P. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics (SAGE, 2018).

  • 78.

    Humle, T. in The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (eds Matsuzawa, T. et al.) 61–72 (Springer, 2011).

  • 79.

    Humle, T. & Matsuzawa, T. Behavioural diversity among the wild chimpanzee populations of Bossou and neighbouring areas, Guinea and Côte d’Ivoire, West Africa. Folia Primatol. 72, 57–68 (2001).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • 80.

    Sugiyama, Y. & Koman, J. Tool-using and making behavior in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20, 513–524 (1979).

    Google Scholar 

  • 81.

    Sugiyama, Y. & Koman, J. A preliminary list of chimpanzees’ alimentation at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 28, 133–147 (1987).

    Google Scholar 

  • 82.

    Joulian, F. in Modelling the Early Human Mind (eds Mellars, P. & Gibson, K.) 173–189 (McDonald Institute Monographs, 1996).

  • 83.

    Matsuzawa, T. & Yamakoshi, G. in Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes (eds Russon, A. E. et al.) 211–232 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1996).


  • Source: Ecology - nature.com

    Hotspots for social and ecological impacts from freshwater stress and storage loss

    MIT Energy Initiative launches the Future Energy Systems Center