in

Social behavior mediates the use of social and personal information in wild jays

  • Gil, M. A., Hein, A. M., Spiegel, O., Baskett, M. L. & Sih, A. Social information links individual behavior to population and community dynamics. Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 535–548 (2018).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior (Oxford University Press, 2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Papini, M. Pattern and process in the evolution of learning. Psychol. Rev. 109, 186–201 (2002).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. H. & Danchin, E. A taxonomy of biological information. Oikos 119, 203–209 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 69, 207–231 (1994).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladds, Z., Hoppitt, W. & Boogert, N. J. Social learning in otters. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4, 170489 (2017).

    ADS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Morand-Ferron, J., Cole, E. F., Rawles, J. E. C. & Quinn, J. L. Who are the innovators? A field experiment with 2 passerine species. Behav. Ecol. 22, 1241–1248 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  • Coussi-Korbel, S. & Fragaszy, D. M. On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Anim. Behav. 50, 1441–1453 (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau, L.-A. & Lefebvre, L. Is social learning an adaptive specialization? In Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture (eds Heyes, C. M. & Galef, B. G.) 107–128 (Academic Press, inc., 1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J. & Templeton, J. J. Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 357, 1559–1566 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M. & Biro, D. Experimental identification of social learning in wild animals. Learn. Behav. 38, 265–283 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. COMMENTARIES is social learning always locally adaptive? Anim. Behav. 52, 637–640 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, H. Conserving and managing animals that learn socially and share cultures. Learn. Behav. 38, 329–336 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenward, B., Rutz, C., Weir, A. A. S. & Kacelnik, A. Development of tool use in New Caledonian crows: Inherited action patterns and social influences. Anim. Behav. 72, 1329–1343 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, J., Stanton, M. A., Patterson, E. M., Bienenstock, E. J. & Singh, L. O. Social networks reveal cultural behaviour in tool-using dolphins. Nat. Commun. 3, 980 (2012).

    ADS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R. & Sanz, C. Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 6, 34783 (2016).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A. & McAuliffe, K. Teaching in wild meerkats. Science 313, 227–229 (2006).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Faegre, S., Nietmann, L., Hannon, P., Ha, J. C. & Ha, R. R. Age-related differences in diet and foraging behavior of the critically endangered Mariana Crow (Corvus kubaryi), with notes on the predation of Coenobita hermit crabs. J. Ornithol. 161, 149–158 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. Social learning strategies. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14 (2004).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W. Machiavellian intelligence. Evol. Anthropol. 5, 172–180 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. What’s social about social learning? J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 193–202 (2012).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, E. H., Avarguès-Weber, A., Chittka, L. & Leadbeater, E. Learning by observation emerges from simple associations in an insect model. Curr. Biol. 23, 727–730 (2013).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolen, I., Giraldeau, L.-A. & Lavoie, M. Head position as an indicator of producer and scrounger tactics in a ground-feeding bird. Anim. Behav. 61, 895–903 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheid, C., Range, F. & Bugnyar, T. When, what, and whom to watch? Quantifying attention in ravens (Corvus corax) and jackdaws (Corvus monedula). J. Comp. Psychol. 121, 380–386 (2007).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models (Princeton University Press, 2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. The burgeoning reach of animal culture. Science 372 (2021).

  • Penn, D. C. & Povinelli, D. J. On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 731–744 (2007).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. Humans are not alone in computing how others see the world. Anim. Behav. 86, 213–221 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, T. R. Social learning mechanisms: Implications for a cognitive theory of imitation. Interact. Stud. 12, 233–261 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  • Akins, C. K. & Zentall, T. R. Imitative learning in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the two-action method. J. Comp. Psychol. 110, 316–320 (1996).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. & Saggerson, A. Testing for imitative and nonimitative social learning in the budgerigar using a two-object/two-action test. Anim. Behav. 64, 851–859 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, W. C. Social and cognitive capabilities of nonhuman primates: Lessons from the wild to captivity. Int. J. Study Anim. Probl. 2, 138–149 (1981).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, B. B., Ward, A. J. W. & Krause, J. Schooling and learning: Early social environment predicts social learning ability in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Anim. Behav. 76, 923–929 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, C. & Taborsky, B. Social experience in early ontogeny has lasting effects on social skills in cooperatively breeding cichlids. Anim. Behav. 79, 621–630 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • McCune, K. B., Jablonski, P. G., Lee, S. & Ha, R. R. Captive jays exhibit reduced problem-solving performance compared to wild conspecifics. R. Soc. Open Sci. 6, 181311 (2019).

    ADS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, A., Kuenstner, K., Mueller, J. & Huber, L. Social learning in a non-social reptile (Geochelone carbonaria). Biol. Lett. 6, 614–616 (2010).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Leadbeater, E. What evolves in the evolution of social learning? J. Zool. 295, 4–11 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Doody, J. S. et al. Aggregated drinking behavior of radiated tortoises (Astrochelys radiata) in arid southwestern Madagascar. Chelonian Conserv. Biol. 10, 145–146 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wendland, L. D. et al. Social behavior drives the dynamics of respiratory disease in threatened tortoises. Ecology 91, 1257–1262 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. & Mesoudi, A. Establishing an experimental science of culture: Animal social diffusion experiments. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 3477–3488 (2008).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Slagsvold, T. & Wiebe, K. L. Social learning in birds and its role in shaping a foraging niche. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 969–977 (2011).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Galef, B. G. & Whiten, A. The comparative psychology of social learning. In APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Vol. 2. Evolution, Development, and Neural Substrate (ed. Call, J.) 411–439 (American Psychological Association, 2017). https://doi.org/10.1037/0000012-019.

    Chapter 

    Google Scholar 

  • Prum, R. O., Robinson, S. K. & Gill, F. B. Ornithology (Macmillan Learning, 2019).

    Google Scholar 

  • Curry, R. L., Townsend Peterson, A. & Langen, T. A. California scrub-jay (Aphelocoma californica). In Birds of North America (eds Poole, A. & Gill, F.) (The Birds of North America, Inc, 2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. L. Mexican jay (Aphelocoma ultramarina). In The Birds of North America (eds Poole, A. & Gill, F.) (The Birds of North America, Inc, 1994).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, N. H., Martínez-Meyer, E. & Peterson, A. T. Ecological niche differentiation in the Aphelocoma jays: A phylogenetic perspective. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 80, 369–383 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  • de Kort, S. R. & Clayton, N. S. An evolutionary perspective on caching by corvids. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 273, 417–423 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pesendorfer, M. B. & Koenig, W. D. Competing for seed dispersal: Evidence for the role of avian seed hoarders in mediating apparent predation among oaks. Funct. Ecol. 31, 622–631 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, T. R. Perspectives on observational learning in animals. J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 114–128 (2012).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Aplin, L. M. et al. Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature 518, 538–541 (2015).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • McCormack, J. E., Jablonski, P. G. & Brown, J. L. Producer-scrounger roles and joining based on dominance in a free-living group of Mexican jays (Aphelocoma ultramarina). Behaviour 144, 967–982 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, C. J., Breen, A. J., Taylor, A. H., Gray, R. D. & Hoppitt, W. How New Caledonian crows solve novel foraging problems and what it means for cumulative culture. Learn. Behav. 44, 18–28 (2016).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, B. J., Thornton, A. & Ridley, A. R. Larger group sizes facilitate the emergence and spread of innovations in a group-living bird. Anim. Behav. 158, 1–7 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, A. S. & Diquelou, M. C. Innovative problem solving in birds: A cross-species comparison of two highly successful passerines. Anim. Behav. 100, 84–94 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Therneau, T. M., Crowson, C. & Atkinson, E. Using time dependent covariates and time dependent coefficents in the Cox model. Survival Vignettes, 2, 3 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, B. J., McElreath, R. L. & Perry, S. E. Pay-off-biased social learning underlies the diffusion of novel extractive foraging traditions in a wild primate. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 284, 20170358 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Therneau, T. M. Coxme: Mixed Effects Cox Models (R Package, 2018).

    Google Scholar 

  • Akaike, H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 19, 716–723 (1974).

    ADS 
    MathSciNet 
    MATH 

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach (Springer, 2002). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2003.11.004.

    Book 
    MATH 

    Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N. S., Dally, J. M. & Emery, N. J. Social cognition by food-caching corvids. The western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 362, 507–522 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B. & Tomasello, M. Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Anim. Behav. 59, 771–785 (2000).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J., Seed, A. M., von Bayern, A. M. P. & Clayton, N. S. Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 489–505 (2007).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Westcott, P. W. Relationships among three species of jays wintering in southeastern Arizona. Condor 71, 353–359 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kulahci, I. G. et al. Social networks predict selective observation and information spread in ravens. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3, 160256 (2016).

    ADS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucherie, P. H., Loretto, M. C., Massen, J. J. M. & Bugnyar, T. What constitutes “social complexity” and “social intelligence” in birds? Lessons from ravens. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73, 12 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J. Cognitive ornithology: The evolution of avian intelligence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 23–43 (2006).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Maclean, E. L. et al. How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Anim. Cogn. 15, 223–238 (2012).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, S. V. & Naeem, S. The phylogenetic component of cooperative breeding in perching birds. Am. Nat. 141, 754–789 (1993).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, E. C., Aldredge, R. A., Peterson, A. T. & McCormack, J. E. New phylogenetic information suggests both an increase and at least one loss of cooperative breeding during the evolutionary history of Aphelocoma jays. Evol. Ecol. 26, 43–54 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, J. & Ericson, P. G. P. Out of Gondwanaland; the evolutionary history of cooperative breeding and social behaviour among crows, magpies, jays and allies. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 273, 1117–1125 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • Midford, P., Hailman, J. & Woolfenden, G. E. Social learning of a novel foraging patch in families of free-living Florida scrub-jays. Anim. Behav. 59, 1199–1207 (2000).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Alcock, J. Animal Behavior (Sinauer Associates, Inc, 2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, J. M., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S. & van Schaik, C. P. Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 129–138 (2012).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? Anim. Cogn. 13, 1–19 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J. & Wagner, R. H. Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305, 487–491 (2004).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil, M. A., Emberts, Z., Jones, H. & St. Mary, C. M. Social information on fear and food drives animal grouping and fitness. Am. Nat. 189, 227–241 (2017).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 187–192 (2008).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. Social cognition. Anim. Behav. 103, 191–202 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Huber, L., Rechberger, S. & Taborsky, M. Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. Anim. Behav. 62, 945–954 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gajdon, G. K., Fijn, N. & Huber, L. Testing social learning in a wild mountain parrot, the kea (Nestor notabilis). Learn. Behav. 32, 62–71 (2004).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • McCowan, B., Anderson, K., Heagarty, A. & Cameron, A. Utility of social network analysis for primate behavioral management and well-being. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 396–405 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, E., Bremner-Harrison, S. & Ward, S. Can we meet the needs of social species in zoos? An overview of the impact of group housing on welfare in socially housed zoo mammals. In Zoo Animals: Husbandry, Welfare and Public Interactions (eds. Berger, M. & Corbett, S.) (Nova Science Publishers, 2018).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoppitt, W., Samson, J., Laland, K. N. & Thornton, A. Identification of learning mechanisms in a wild Meerkat population. PLoS ONE 7, 1–11 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, R. L., Galef, B. G. & van Schaik, C. P. Social learning research outside the laboratory: How and why? Learn. Behav. 38, 187–194 (2010).

    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A. & Lukas, D. Individual variation in cognitive performance: Developmental and evolutionary perspectives. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 2773–2783 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317, 1360–1366 (2007).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda, R. P. & Kamil, A. C. Spatial and social cognition in corvids: An evolutionary approach. In The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition (eds. Bekoff, M., Burghardt, G. & Allen, C.) (Bradford Book, 2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • Greggor, A. L., Thornton, A. & Clayton, N. S. Harnessing learning biases is essential for applying social learning in conservation. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 71, 1–12 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brakes, P. et al. A deepening understanding of animal culture suggests lessons for conservation. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 288, 20202718 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  • Brakes, P. et al. Animal cultures matter for conservation. Science 363, 1032–1034 (2019).

    ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, B. J., Zepeda, E., Pollack, L., Munson, A. & Sih, A. Counter-culture: Does social learning help or hinder adaptive response to human-induced rapid environmental change? Front. Ecol. Evol. 7, 1–18 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rushworth, M. F. S., Mars, R. B. & Sallet, J. Are there specialized circuits for social cognition and are they unique to humans? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 436–442 (2013).

    CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. & Burkart, J. M. Social learning and evolution: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1008–1016 (2011).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 


  • Source: Ecology - nature.com

    Solar-powered system offers a route to inexpensive desalination

    Nurturing human communities and natural ecosystems