Lipkind, D. et al. Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature 498, 104–108 (2013).
Google Scholar
Goldstein, M., King, A. P. & West, M. J. Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 8030–8035 (2003).
Google Scholar
Fehér, O., Ljubičić, I., Suzuki, K., Okanoya, K. & Tchernichovski, O. Statistical learning in songbirds: from self-tutoring to song culture. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160053 (2017).
Google Scholar
Tchernichovski, O., Lints, T., Mitra, P. P. & Nottebohm, F. Vocal imitation in zebra finches is inversely related to model abundance. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 12901–12904 (1999).
Google Scholar
Tchernichovski, O. Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science 291, 2564–2569 (2001).
Google Scholar
Fehér, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P. & Tchernichovski, O. De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature 459, 564–568 (2009).
Google Scholar
Takahashi, D. et al. The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science 349, 734–738 (2015).
Google Scholar
Takahashi, D. Y., Liao, D. A. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Vocal learning via social reinforcement by infant marmoset monkeys. Curr. Biol. 27, 1844–1852.E6 (2017).
Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371, 20150370 (2016).
Google Scholar
Gultekin, Y. B. & Hage, S. R. Limiting parental interaction during vocal development affects acoustic call structure in marmoset monkeys. Sci. Adv. 4, eaar4012 (2018).
Google Scholar
Gultekin, Y. B. & Hage, S. R. Limiting parental feedback disrupts vocal development in marmoset monkeys. Nat. Commun. 8, 14046 (2017).
Google Scholar
Jarvis, E. D. Evolution of vocal learning and spoken language. Science 366, 50–54 (2019).
Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. Learning from monkey “talk”. Science 355, 1120–1122 (2017).
Google Scholar
Malik, K. Rights and wrongs. Nature 406, 675–676 (2000).
Wise, S. M. & Goodall, J. Rattling the Cage: Toward Legal Rights for Animals (Da Capo Press, 2017).
Grayson, L. Animals in Research: For and Against (British Library, 2000).
Nater, A. et al. Morphometric, behavioral, and genomic evidence for a new orangutan species. Curr. Biol. 27, 3487–3498.E10 (2017).
Google Scholar
Estrada, A. et al. Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: why primates matter. Sci. Adv. 3, e1600946 (2017).
Google Scholar
Ross, S. et al. Inappropriate use and portrayal of chimpanzees. Science 319, 1487 (2008).
Google Scholar
Wich, S. A. et al. Land-cover changes predict steep declines for the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii). Sci. Adv. 2, e1500789 (2016).
Google Scholar
Wich, S. A. et al. Understanding the impacts of land-use policies on a threatened species: is there a future for the Bornean orangutan? PLoS ONE 7, e49142 (2012).
Google Scholar
Wich, S. A. et al. Will oil palm’s homecoming spell doom for Africa’s great apes? Curr. Biol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.077 (2014).
Fitch, T. W. Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 3–33 (2017).
Hauser, M. D. et al. The mystery of language evolution. Front. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00401 (2014)
Corballis, M. C. Crossing the Rubicon: behaviorism, language, and evolutionary continuity. Front. Psychol. 11, 653 (2020).
Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C. & Chomsky, N. All or nothing: no half-merge and the evolution of syntax. PLoS Biol. 17, e3000539 (2019).
Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. & Wynne, C. D. Can evolution explain how minds work? Nature 458, 832–833 (2009).
Google Scholar
Hayes, K. J. & Hayes, C. The intellectual development of a home-raised chimpanzee. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc. 95, 105–109 (1951).
Premack, D. Language in chimpanzee? Science 172, 808–822 (1971).
Google Scholar
Terrace, H., Petitto, L., Sanders, R. & Bever, T. Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206, 891–902 (1979).
Google Scholar
Patterson, F. & Linden, E. The Education of Koko (Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1981).
Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A. & Hopkins, W. D. BIZARRE chimpanzees do not represent “the chimpanzee”. Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 100–101 (2010).
Lameira, A. R. Bidding evidence for primate vocal learning and the cultural substrates for speech evolution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83, 429–439 (2017).
Lameira, A. R. et al. Speech-like rhythm in a voiced and voiceless orangutan call. PLoS ONE 10, e116136 (2015).
Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R. & Shumaker, R. W. Orangutans show active voicing through a membranophone. Sci. Rep. 9, 12289 (2019).
Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Mielke, A., Wich, S. A. & Shumaker, R. W. Vocal fold control beyond the species-specific repertoire in an orangutan. Sci. Rep. 6, 30315 (2016).
Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R. et al. Orangutan (Pongo spp.) whistling and implications for the emergence of an open-ended call repertoire: a replication and extension. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134, 2326–2335 (2013).
Perlman, M. & Clark, N. Learned vocal and breathing behavior in an enculturated gorilla. Anim. Cogn. 18, 1165–1179 (2015).
Wich, S. et al. A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan. Primates 50, 56–64 (2009).
Lameira, A. R., Maddieson, I. & Zuberbuhler, K. Primate feedstock for the evolution of consonants. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 60–62 (2014).
Lameira, A. R. The forgotten role of consonant-like calls in theories of speech evolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 559–560 (2014).
Boë, L.-J. et al. Which way to the dawn of speech? Reanalyzing half a century of debates and data in light of speech science. Sci. Adv. 5, eaaw3916 (2019).
Google Scholar
Boë, L. J. et al. Evidence of a vocalic proto-system in the baboon (Papio papio) suggests pre-hominin speech precursors. PLoS ONE 12, e0169321 (2017).
Google Scholar
Fitch, T. W., Boer, B., Mathur, N. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready. Sci. Adv. 2, e1600723 (2016).
Google Scholar
Pereira, A. S., Kavanagh, E., Hobaiter, C., Slocombe, K. E. & Lameira, A. R. Chimpanzee lip-smacks confirm primate continuity for speech-rhythm evolution. Biol. Lett. 16, 20200232 (2020).
Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R. et al. Proto-consonants were information-dense via identical bioacoustic tags to proto-vowels. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0044 (2017).
Lameira, A. R. et al. Orangutan information broadcast via consonant-like and vowel-like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution. Biol. Lett. 17, 20210302 (2021).
Google Scholar
Watson, S. K. et al. Nonadjacent dependency processing in monkeys, apes, and humans. Sci. Adv. 6, eabb0725 (2020).
Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R. & Call, J. Time-space–displaced responses in the orangutan vocal system. Sci. Adv. 4, eaau3401 (2018).
Google Scholar
Belyk, M. & Brown, S. The origins of the vocal brain in humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77, 177–193 (2017).
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M. & Zuberbuhler, K. Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social-cognitive processes. Sci. Adv. 3, e1701742 (2017).
Google Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Reamer, L., Schapiro, S. J. & Hopkins, W. D. Social learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees. Biol. Lett. 8, 498–501 (2012).
Google Scholar
Russell, J. L., Joseph, M., Hopkins, W. D. & Taglialatela, J. P. Vocal learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Brain Lang. 127, 520–525 (2013).
Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D. et al. Genetic factors and orofacial motor learning selectively influence variability in central sulcus morphology in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Neurosci. 37, 5475–5483 (2017).
Google Scholar
Staes, N. et al. FOXP2 variation in great ape populations offers insight into the evolution of communication skills. Sci. Rep. 7, 16866 (2017).
Google Scholar
Martins, P. T. & Boeckx, C. Vocal learning: beyond the continuum. PLoS Biol. 18, e3000672 (2020).
Google Scholar
Watson, S. K. et al. Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 25, 495–499 (2015).
Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. & Leavens, D. A. Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim. Behav. 73, 281–286 (2007).
Google Scholar
Bianchi, S., Reyes, L. D., Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. & Sherwood, C. C. Neocortical grey matter distribution underlying voluntary, flexible vocalizations in chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 6, 34733 (2016).
Google Scholar
Wich, S. A. et al. Call cultures in orangutans? PLoS ONE 7, e36180 (2012).
Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L. & Boesch, C. Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110, 221–243 (2004).
Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685 (1999).
Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. et al. Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299, 102–105 (2003).
Whiten, A. Culture extends the scope of evolutionary biology in the great apes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 7790–7797 (2017).
Google Scholar
Koops, K., Visalberghi, E. & van Schaik, C. The ecology of primate material culture. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140508 (2014).
Google Scholar
Kalan, A. K. et al. Chimpanzees use tree species with a resonant timbre for accumulative stone throwing. Biol. Lett. 15, 20190747 (2019).
Google Scholar
Hardus, M., Lameira, A. R., Van Schaik, C. P. & Wich, S. A. Tool use in wild orangutans modifies sound production: a functionally deceptive innovation? Proc. R. Soc. B https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1027 (2009).
Lameira, A. R. et al. Population-specific use of the same tool-assisted alarm call between two wild orangutan populations (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) indicates functional arbitrariness. PLoS ONE 8, e69749 (2013).
Google Scholar
Hohmann, G. & Fruth, B. Culture in bonobos? Between‐species and within‐species variation in behavior. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 563–571 (2003).
Robbins, M. M. et al. Behavioral variation in gorillas: evidence of potential cultural traits. PLoS ONE 11, e0160483 (2016).
Google Scholar
Kühl, H. S. et al. Human impact erodes chimpanzee behavioral diversity. Science 363, 1453–1455 (2019).
van Schaik, C. P. Fragility of Traditions: the disturbance hypothesis for the loss of local traditions in orangutans. Int. J. Primatol. 23, 527–538 (2002).
Delgado, R. A. & van Schaik, C. P. The behavioral ecology and conservation of the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus): a tale of two islands. Evol. Anthropol. 9, 201–218 (2000).
van Schaik, C. The socioecology of fission–fusion sociality in orangutans. Primates 40, 69–86 (1999).
Nater, A. et al. Sex-biased dispersal and volcanic activities shaped phylogeographic patterns of extant orangutans (genus: Pongo). Mol. Biol. 28, 2275–2288 (2011).
Google Scholar
Arora, N. et al. Parentage-based pedigree reconstruction reveals female matrilineal clusters and male-biased dispersal in nongregarious Asian great apes, the Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Mol. Ecol. 21, 3352–3362 (2012).
Google Scholar
Kavanagh, E. et al. Dominance style is a key predictor of vocal use and evolution across nonhuman primates. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8, 210873 (2021).
Google Scholar
Husson, S. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 6 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).
van Noordwijk, M. A. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 12 (Oxford Univ Press, 2009).
Singleton, I., Knott, C., Morrogh-Bernard, H., Wich, S. & van Schaik, C. P. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 13 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).
Wich, S. et al. Life history of wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii). J. Hum. Evol. 47, 385–398 (2004).
Google Scholar
Wich, S. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 5 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).
Shumaker, R. W., Wich, S. A. & Perkins, L. Reproductive life history traits of female orangutans (Pongo spp.). Primate Reprod. Aging 36, 147–161 (2008).
Google Scholar
Freund, C., Rahman, E. & Knott, C. Ten years of orangutan-related wildlife crime investigation in West Kalimantan, Indonesia. Am. J. Primatol. 79, 22620 (2016).
van Noordwijk, M. A. & van Schaik, C. P. Development of ecological competence in Sumatran orangutans. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127, 79–94 (2005).
Knot, C. D. et al. The Gunung Palung Orangutan Project: Twenty-five years at the intersection of research and conservation in a critical landscape in Indonesia. Biol. Conserv. 255, 108856 (2021).
Guillermo, S.-B., Gershenson, C. & Fernández, N. A package for measuring emergence, self-organization, and complexity based on shannon entropy. Front. Robot. AI 4, 174102 (2017).
Santamaría-Bonfil, G., Fernández, N. & Gershenson, C. Measuring the complexity of continuous distributions. Entropy 18, 72 (2016).
Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. Wild chimpanzees modify food call structure with respect to tree size for a particular fruit species. Anim. Behav. 101, 1–9 (2015).
Fedurek, P. & Slocombe, K. E. The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 726–739 (2013).
Luef, E., Breuer, T. & Pika, S. Food-associated calling in gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla) in the wild. PLoS ONE 11, e0144197 (2016).
Google Scholar
Clay, Z. & Zuberbuhler, K. Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos. Anim. Behav. 77, 1387–1396 (2009).
Hardus, M. E. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 4 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).
Wich, S. A. et al. Forest fruit production is higher on Sumatra than on Borneo. PLoS ONE 6, e21278 (2011).
Google Scholar
Lameira, A. R. & Wich, S. Orangutan long call degradation and individuality over distance: a playback approach. Int. J. Primatol. 29, 615–625 (2008).
Lameira, A. R., Delgado, R. & Wich, S. Review of geographic variation in terrestrial mammalian acoustic signals: human speech variation in a comparative perspective. J. Evolut. Psychol. 8, 309–332 (2010).
Lameira, A. R. et al. Predator guild does not influence orangutan alarm call rates and combinations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 519–528 (2013).
Derex, M. & Mesoudi, A. Cumulative cultural evolution within evolving population structures. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 654–667 (2020).
Scerri, E. M. et al. Did our species evolve in subdivided populations across Africa, and why does it matter? Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 582–594 (2018).
Google Scholar
Kaya, F. et al. The rise and fall of the Old World savannah fauna and the origins of the African savannah biome. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 241–246 (2018).
Google Scholar
Bobe, R. The expansion of grassland ecosystems in Africa in relation to mammalian evolution and the origin of the genus Homo. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 207, 399–420 (2004).
Zhu, D., Galbraith, E. D., Reyes-García, V. & Ciais, P. Global hunter-gatherer population densities constrained by influence of seasonality on diet composition. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 5, 1536–1545 (2021).
Google Scholar
DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A. & Higham, J. P. Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 0112 (2017).
Mauricio, G.-F. & Gardner, A. Inference of ecological and social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature 557, 554–557 (2018).
Lindenfors, P., Wartel, A. & Lind, J. ‘Dunbar’s number’ deconstructed. Biol. Lett. 17, 20210158 (2021).
Google Scholar
Schuppli, C., van Noordwijk, M., Atmoko, S. U. & van Schaik, C. Early sociability fosters later exploratory tendency in wild immature orangutans. Sci. Adv. 6, eaaw2685 (2020).
Google Scholar
Schuppli, C. et al. Observational social learning and socially induced practice of routine skills in immature wild orangutans. Anim. Behav. 119, 87–98 (2016).
Jaeggi, A. V. et al. Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: implications for culture. Am. J. Primatol. 72, 62–71 (2010).
Google Scholar
Schuppli, C. et al. The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Sci. Rep. 7, 15464 (2017).
Google Scholar
Ehmann, B. et al. Immature wild orangutans acquire relevant ecological knowledge through sex-specific attentional biases during social learning. PLoS Biol. 19, e3001173 (2021).
Google Scholar
Meijaard, E. et al. Declining orangutan encounter rates from Wallace to the present suggest the species was once more abundant. PLoS ONE 5, e12042 (2010).
Google Scholar
Marshall, A. J. et al. The blowgun is mightier than the chainsaw in determining population density of Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) in the forests of East Kalimantan. Biol. Conserv. 129, 566–578 (2006).
Gail, C.-S., Miran, C.-S., Singleton, I. & Linkie, M. Raiders of the lost bark: orangutan foraging strategies in a degraded landscape. PLoS ONE 6, e20962 (2011).
Schuppli, C. & van Schaik, C. P. Animal cultures: how we’ve only seen the tip of the iceberg. Evol. Hum. Sci. 1, e2 (2019).
Langergraber, K. E. et al. Vigilant, generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 15716–15721 (2012).
Google Scholar
Fernández, N., Maldonado, C. & Gershenson, C. in Guided Self-Organization: Inception (ed Prokopenko, M.) 19–51 (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2014).
JAST Team, JASP (Univ. of Amsterdam, 2020).
R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2013).
Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis (Springer-Verlag, 2009).
Auguie, B. gridExtra: Functions in grid graphics. R version 0.9.1 (2012).
Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300 (1995).
Korthauer, K. et al. A practical guide to methods controlling false discoveries in computational biology. Genome Biol. 20, 118 (2019).
Google Scholar
Source: Ecology - nature.com