in

Sociality predicts orangutan vocal phenotype

  • Lipkind, D. et al. Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature 498, 104–108 (2013).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M., King, A. P. & West, M. J. Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 8030–8035 (2003).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehér, O., Ljubičić, I., Suzuki, K., Okanoya, K. & Tchernichovski, O. Statistical learning in songbirds: from self-tutoring to song culture. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160053 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Tchernichovski, O., Lints, T., Mitra, P. P. & Nottebohm, F. Vocal imitation in zebra finches is inversely related to model abundance. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 12901–12904 (1999).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Tchernichovski, O. Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science 291, 2564–2569 (2001).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehér, O., Wang, H., Saar, S., Mitra, P. P. & Tchernichovski, O. De novo establishment of wild-type song culture in the zebra finch. Nature 459, 564–568 (2009).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, D. et al. The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science 349, 734–738 (2015).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, D. Y., Liao, D. A. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Vocal learning via social reinforcement by infant marmoset monkeys. Curr. Biol. 27, 1844–1852.E6 (2017).

  • Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371, 20150370 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Gultekin, Y. B. & Hage, S. R. Limiting parental interaction during vocal development affects acoustic call structure in marmoset monkeys. Sci. Adv. 4, eaar4012 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Gultekin, Y. B. & Hage, S. R. Limiting parental feedback disrupts vocal development in marmoset monkeys. Nat. Commun. 8, 14046 (2017).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, E. D. Evolution of vocal learning and spoken language. Science 366, 50–54 (2019).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T. Learning from monkey “talk”. Science 355, 1120–1122 (2017).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Malik, K. Rights and wrongs. Nature 406, 675–676 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wise, S. M. & Goodall, J. Rattling the Cage: Toward Legal Rights for Animals (Da Capo Press, 2017).

  • Grayson, L. Animals in Research: For and Against (British Library, 2000).

  • Nater, A. et al. Morphometric, behavioral, and genomic evidence for a new orangutan species. Curr. Biol. 27, 3487–3498.E10 (2017).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Estrada, A. et al. Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: why primates matter. Sci. Adv. 3, e1600946 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, S. et al. Inappropriate use and portrayal of chimpanzees. Science 319, 1487 (2008).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A. et al. Land-cover changes predict steep declines for the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo abelii). Sci. Adv. 2, e1500789 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A. et al. Understanding the impacts of land-use policies on a threatened species: is there a future for the Bornean orangutan? PLoS ONE 7, e49142 (2012).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A. et al. Will oil palm’s homecoming spell doom for Africa’s great apes? Curr. Biol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.077 (2014).

  • Fitch, T. W. Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 3–33 (2017).

  • Hauser, M. D. et al. The mystery of language evolution. Front. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00401 (2014)

  • Corballis, M. C. Crossing the Rubicon: behaviorism, language, and evolutionary continuity. Front. Psychol. 11, 653 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick, R. C. & Chomsky, N. All or nothing: no half-merge and the evolution of syntax. PLoS Biol. 17, e3000539 (2019).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolhuis, J. J. & Wynne, C. D. Can evolution explain how minds work? Nature 458, 832–833 (2009).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, K. J. & Hayes, C. The intellectual development of a home-raised chimpanzee. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc. 95, 105–109 (1951).

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. Language in chimpanzee? Science 172, 808–822 (1971).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H., Petitto, L., Sanders, R. & Bever, T. Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206, 891–902 (1979).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, F. & Linden, E. The Education of Koko (Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1981).

  • Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A. & Hopkins, W. D. BIZARRE chimpanzees do not represent “the chimpanzee”. Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 100–101 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. Bidding evidence for primate vocal learning and the cultural substrates for speech evolution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 83, 429–439 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. et al. Speech-like rhythm in a voiced and voiceless orangutan call. PLoS ONE 10, e116136 (2015).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. & Shumaker, R. W. Orangutans show active voicing through a membranophone. Sci. Rep. 9, 12289 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R., Hardus, M. E., Mielke, A., Wich, S. A. & Shumaker, R. W. Vocal fold control beyond the species-specific repertoire in an orangutan. Sci. Rep. 6, 30315 (2016).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. et al. Orangutan (Pongo spp.) whistling and implications for the emergence of an open-ended call repertoire: a replication and extension. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 134, 2326–2335 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlman, M. & Clark, N. Learned vocal and breathing behavior in an enculturated gorilla. Anim. Cogn. 18, 1165–1179 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. et al. A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan. Primates 50, 56–64 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R., Maddieson, I. & Zuberbuhler, K. Primate feedstock for the evolution of consonants. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 60–62 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. The forgotten role of consonant-like calls in theories of speech evolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 559–560 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  • Boë, L.-J. et al. Which way to the dawn of speech? Reanalyzing half a century of debates and data in light of speech science. Sci. Adv. 5, eaaw3916 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Boë, L. J. et al. Evidence of a vocalic proto-system in the baboon (Papio papio) suggests pre-hominin speech precursors. PLoS ONE 12, e0169321 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, T. W., Boer, B., Mathur, N. & Ghazanfar, A. A. Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready. Sci. Adv. 2, e1600723 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Pereira, A. S., Kavanagh, E., Hobaiter, C., Slocombe, K. E. & Lameira, A. R. Chimpanzee lip-smacks confirm primate continuity for speech-rhythm evolution. Biol. Lett. 16, 20200232 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. et al. Proto-consonants were information-dense via identical bioacoustic tags to proto-vowels. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0044 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. et al. Orangutan information broadcast via consonant-like and vowel-like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution. Biol. Lett. 17, 20210302 (2021).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, S. K. et al. Nonadjacent dependency processing in monkeys, apes, and humans. Sci. Adv. 6, eabb0725 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. & Call, J. Time-space–displaced responses in the orangutan vocal system. Sci. Adv. 4, eaau3401 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Belyk, M. & Brown, S. The origins of the vocal brain in humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77, 177–193 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M. & Zuberbuhler, K. Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social-cognitive processes. Sci. Adv. 3, e1701742 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Taglialatela, J. P., Reamer, L., Schapiro, S. J. & Hopkins, W. D. Social learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees. Biol. Lett. 8, 498–501 (2012).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. L., Joseph, M., Hopkins, W. D. & Taglialatela, J. P. Vocal learning of a communicative signal in captive chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Brain Lang. 127, 520–525 (2013).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W. D. et al. Genetic factors and orofacial motor learning selectively influence variability in central sulcus morphology in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Neurosci. 37, 5475–5483 (2017).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Staes, N. et al. FOXP2 variation in great ape populations offers insight into the evolution of communication skills. Sci. Rep. 7, 16866 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Martins, P. T. & Boeckx, C. Vocal learning: beyond the continuum. PLoS Biol. 18, e3000672 (2020).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, S. K. et al. Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 25, 495–499 (2015).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. & Leavens, D. A. Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim. Behav. 73, 281–286 (2007).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi, S., Reyes, L. D., Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P. & Sherwood, C. C. Neocortical grey matter distribution underlying voluntary, flexible vocalizations in chimpanzees. Sci. Rep. 6, 34733 (2016).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. A. et al. Call cultures in orangutans? PLoS ONE 7, e36180 (2012).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L. & Boesch, C. Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110, 221–243 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685 (1999).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. et al. Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299, 102–105 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. Culture extends the scope of evolutionary biology in the great apes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 7790–7797 (2017).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Koops, K., Visalberghi, E. & van Schaik, C. The ecology of primate material culture. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140508 (2014).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalan, A. K. et al. Chimpanzees use tree species with a resonant timbre for accumulative stone throwing. Biol. Lett. 15, 20190747 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardus, M., Lameira, A. R., Van Schaik, C. P. & Wich, S. A. Tool use in wild orangutans modifies sound production: a functionally deceptive innovation? Proc. R. Soc. B https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1027 (2009).

  • Lameira, A. R. et al. Population-specific use of the same tool-assisted alarm call between two wild orangutan populations (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) indicates functional arbitrariness. PLoS ONE 8, e69749 (2013).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G. & Fruth, B. Culture in bonobos? Between‐species and within‐species variation in behavior. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 563–571 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, M. M. et al. Behavioral variation in gorillas: evidence of potential cultural traits. PLoS ONE 11, e0160483 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Kühl, H. S. et al. Human impact erodes chimpanzee behavioral diversity. Science 363, 1453–1455 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. Fragility of Traditions: the disturbance hypothesis for the loss of local traditions in orangutans. Int. J. Primatol. 23, 527–538 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  • Delgado, R. A. & van Schaik, C. P. The behavioral ecology and conservation of the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus): a tale of two islands. Evol. Anthropol. 9, 201–218 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. The socioecology of fission–fusion sociality in orangutans. Primates 40, 69–86 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nater, A. et al. Sex-biased dispersal and volcanic activities shaped phylogeographic patterns of extant orangutans (genus: Pongo). Mol. Biol. 28, 2275–2288 (2011).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Arora, N. et al. Parentage-based pedigree reconstruction reveals female matrilineal clusters and male-biased dispersal in nongregarious Asian great apes, the Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Mol. Ecol. 21, 3352–3362 (2012).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Kavanagh, E. et al. Dominance style is a key predictor of vocal use and evolution across nonhuman primates. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8, 210873 (2021).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Husson, S. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 6 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

  • van Noordwijk, M. A. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 12 (Oxford Univ Press, 2009).

  • Singleton, I., Knott, C., Morrogh-Bernard, H., Wich, S. & van Schaik, C. P. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 13 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

  • Wich, S. et al. Life history of wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii). J. Hum. Evol. 47, 385–398 (2004).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Wich, S. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 5 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

  • Shumaker, R. W., Wich, S. A. & Perkins, L. Reproductive life history traits of female orangutans (Pongo spp.). Primate Reprod. Aging 36, 147–161 (2008).

    CAS 

    Google Scholar 

  • Freund, C., Rahman, E. & Knott, C. Ten years of orangutan-related wildlife crime investigation in West Kalimantan, Indonesia. Am. J. Primatol. 79, 22620 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  • van Noordwijk, M. A. & van Schaik, C. P. Development of ecological competence in Sumatran orangutans. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127, 79–94 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  • Knot, C. D. et al. The Gunung Palung Orangutan Project: Twenty-five years at the intersection of research and conservation in a critical landscape in Indonesia. Biol. Conserv. 255, 108856 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillermo, S.-B., Gershenson, C. & Fernández, N. A package for measuring emergence, self-organization, and complexity based on shannon entropy. Front. Robot. AI 4, 174102 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Santamaría-Bonfil, G., Fernández, N. & Gershenson, C. Measuring the complexity of continuous distributions. Entropy 18, 72 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. Wild chimpanzees modify food call structure with respect to tree size for a particular fruit species. Anim. Behav. 101, 1–9 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fedurek, P. & Slocombe, K. E. The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 726–739 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • Luef, E., Breuer, T. & Pika, S. Food-associated calling in gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla) in the wild. PLoS ONE 11, e0144197 (2016).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Clay, Z. & Zuberbuhler, K. Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos. Anim. Behav. 77, 1387–1396 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardus, M. E. et al. in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (eds Wich, S. et al.) Ch. 4 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2009).

  • Wich, S. A. et al. Forest fruit production is higher on Sumatra than on Borneo. PLoS ONE 6, e21278 (2011).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. & Wich, S. Orangutan long call degradation and individuality over distance: a playback approach. Int. J. Primatol. 29, 615–625 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R., Delgado, R. & Wich, S. Review of geographic variation in terrestrial mammalian acoustic signals: human speech variation in a comparative perspective. J. Evolut. Psychol. 8, 309–332 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira, A. R. et al. Predator guild does not influence orangutan alarm call rates and combinations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 519–528 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  • Derex, M. & Mesoudi, A. Cumulative cultural evolution within evolving population structures. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 654–667 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  • Scerri, E. M. et al. Did our species evolve in subdivided populations across Africa, and why does it matter? Trends Ecol. Evol. 33, 582–594 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaya, F. et al. The rise and fall of the Old World savannah fauna and the origins of the African savannah biome. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 241–246 (2018).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobe, R. The expansion of grassland ecosystems in Africa in relation to mammalian evolution and the origin of the genus Homo. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 207, 399–420 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, D., Galbraith, E. D., Reyes-García, V. & Ciais, P. Global hunter-gatherer population densities constrained by influence of seasonality on diet composition. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 5, 1536–1545 (2021).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A. & Higham, J. P. Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 0112 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauricio, G.-F. & Gardner, A. Inference of ecological and social drivers of human brain-size evolution. Nature 557, 554–557 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindenfors, P., Wartel, A. & Lind, J. ‘Dunbar’s number’ deconstructed. Biol. Lett. 17, 20210158 (2021).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuppli, C., van Noordwijk, M., Atmoko, S. U. & van Schaik, C. Early sociability fosters later exploratory tendency in wild immature orangutans. Sci. Adv. 6, eaaw2685 (2020).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuppli, C. et al. Observational social learning and socially induced practice of routine skills in immature wild orangutans. Anim. Behav. 119, 87–98 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, A. V. et al. Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: implications for culture. Am. J. Primatol. 72, 62–71 (2010).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuppli, C. et al. The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Sci. Rep. 7, 15464 (2017).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehmann, B. et al. Immature wild orangutans acquire relevant ecological knowledge through sex-specific attentional biases during social learning. PLoS Biol. 19, e3001173 (2021).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Meijaard, E. et al. Declining orangutan encounter rates from Wallace to the present suggest the species was once more abundant. PLoS ONE 5, e12042 (2010).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, A. J. et al. The blowgun is mightier than the chainsaw in determining population density of Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) in the forests of East Kalimantan. Biol. Conserv. 129, 566–578 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gail, C.-S., Miran, C.-S., Singleton, I. & Linkie, M. Raiders of the lost bark: orangutan foraging strategies in a degraded landscape. PLoS ONE 6, e20962 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuppli, C. & van Schaik, C. P. Animal cultures: how we’ve only seen the tip of the iceberg. Evol. Hum. Sci. 1, e2 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  • Langergraber, K. E. et al. Vigilant, generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 15716–15721 (2012).

    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, N., Maldonado, C. & Gershenson, C. in Guided Self-Organization: Inception (ed Prokopenko, M.) 19–51 (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2014).

  • JAST Team, JASP (Univ. of Amsterdam, 2020).

  • R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2013).

  • Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis (Springer-Verlag, 2009).

  • Auguie, B. gridExtra: Functions in grid graphics. R version 0.9.1 (2012).

  • Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300 (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  • Korthauer, K. et al. A practical guide to methods controlling false discoveries in computational biology. Genome Biol. 20, 118 (2019).

    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 


  • Source: Ecology - nature.com

    European-wide forest monitoring substantiate the neccessity for a joint conservation strategy to rescue European ash species (Fraxinus spp.)

    Finding her way to fusion