Stiner, M. C. The challenges of documenting coevolution and niche construction: The example of domestic spaces. Evol. Anthropol. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21878 (2020).
Google Scholar
Potts, R. Why the Oldowan? Plio-Pleistocene toolmaking and the transport of resources. J. Anthropol. Res. 47, 153–176 (1991).
Kuhn, S. L., Raichlen, D. A. & Clark, A. E. What moves us? How mobility and movement are at the center of human evolution. Evol. Anthropol. 25, 86–97 (2016).
Google Scholar
Haas, R. & Kuhn, S. L. Forager mobility in constructed environments. Curr. Anthropol. 60, 499–535 (2019).
Iovita, R. et al. Operationalizing niche construction theory with stone tools. Evol. Anthropol. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21881 (2021).
Google Scholar
Reeves, J. S., Braun, D. R., Finestone, E. M. & Plummer, T. W. Ecological perspectives on technological diversity at Kanjera South. J. Hum. Evol. 158, 103029 (2021).
Google Scholar
Finestone, E. M., Braun, D. R., Plummer, T. W., Bartilol, S. & Kiprono, N. Building ED-XRF datasets for sourcing rhyolite and quartzite artifacts: A case study on the Homa Peninsula, Kenya. J. Archaeol. Sci. 33, 102510 (2020).
Braun, D. R. et al. Oldowan behavior and raw material transport: Perspectives from the Kanjera Formation. J. Archaeol. Sci. 35, 2329–2345 (2008).
Potts, R. Home bases and early hominids. Am. Sci. 72, 338–347 (1984).
Google Scholar
Schick, K. D. Modeling the formation of Early Stone Age artifact concentrations. J. Hum. Evol. 16, 789–807 (1987).
Binford, L. R. Willow smoke and dogs’ Tails: Hunter-gatherer settlement systems and archaeological site formation. Am. Antiq. 45, 4–20 (1980).
Schiffer, M. B. Archaeology as behavioral science. Am. Anthropol. 77, 836–848 (1975).
Schiffer, M. B. Formation Processes of the Archaeological Record (University of New Mexico Press, 1987).
Binford, L. R. Behavioral Archaeology and the ‘Pompeii Premise’. J. Anthropol. Res. 37, 195–208 (1981).
Binford, L. R. The archaeology of place. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 1, 5–31 (1982).
Braun, D. R. et al. Ecosystem engineering in the Quaternary of the West Coast of South Africa. Evol. Anthropol. 30, 50–62 (2020).
Yellen, J. E. Archaeological Approaches to the Present: Models for Reconstructing the Past (Academic Press, 1977).
Isaac, G. L. L. The Harvey Lecture Series, 1977–1978. Food sharing and human evolution: Archaeological Evidence from the Plio-Pleistocene of East Africa Author (s): Glynn Ll Isaac Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable. http://www.jstor.org/sta. 34, 311–325 (1978).
Brooks, A. S. & Yellen, J. E. The preservation of activity areas in the archaeological record: Ethnoarchaeological and archaeological work in NOrthwest Ngamiland, Botswana. In Methog and Theory for Activity Area Research: An Ethnoarchaeological Approach 63–106 (Columbia University Press, 1987).
Binford, L. R. Nunamiut Ethnoarchaeology (Percheron Press, 2012).
Hawkes, K. Ethnoarchaeology and Plio-Pleistocene sites: Some lessons from the Hadza. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 44, 158–165 (2016).
McGrew, W. Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution (Cambridge University Press, 1992).
Carvalho, S., Cunha, E., Sousa, C. & Matsuzawa, T. Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking. J. Hum. Evol. 55, 148–163 (2008).
Google Scholar
Whiten, A. Archaeology meets primate technology. Nature 498, 303–305 (2013).
Google Scholar
Haslam, M. et al. Primate archaeology evolves. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1431–1437 (2017).
Google Scholar
Biro, D., Haslam, M. & Rutz, C. Tool use as adaptation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 368, 20120408 (2013).
Carvalho, S., Biro, D., McGrew, W. C. & Matsuzawa, T. Tool-composite reuse in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early hominins?. Anim. Cogn. 12, 103–114 (2009).
Haslam, M. et al. Primate archaeology. Nature 460, 339–344 (2009).
Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates 25, 160–170 (1984).
Hannah, A. C. & McGrew, W. C. Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia. Primates 28, 31–46 (1987).
Luncz, L. V., Proffitt, T., Kulik, L., Haslam, M. & Wittig, R. M. Distance-decay effect in stone tool transport by wild chimpanzees. Proc. R. Soc. B 283, 20161607 (2016).
Google Scholar
Braun, D. R., Harris, J. W. K. & Maina, D. N. Oldowan raw material procurement and use: Evidence from the koobi fora formation. Archaeometry 51, 26–42 (2009).
Google Scholar
Plummer, T. W. Flaked stones and old bones: Biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 47, 118–164 (2004).
Isaac, G. The archaeology of human origins: Studies of the Lower Pleistocene in East Africa, 1971–1981. Adv. World Archaeol. 3, 1–86 (1984).
Blumenschine, R. J., Masao, F. T., Tactikos, J. C. & Ebert, J. I. Effects of distance from stone source on landscape-scale variation in Oldowan artifact assemblages in the Paleo-Olduvai Basin, Tanzania. J. Archaeol. Sci. 35, 76–86 (2008).
Blumenschine, R. J. et al. Landscape distribution of Oldowan stone artifact assemblages across the fault compartments of the eastern Olduvai Lake Basin during early lowermost Bed II times. J. Hum. Evol. 63, 384–394 (2012).
Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. et al. Distribution of potential suitable hammers and transport of hammer tools and nuts by wild capuchin monkeys. Primates 50, 95–104 (2009).
Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M. et al. The fourth dimension of tool use: Temporally enduring artefacts aid primates learning to use tools. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 368, 20120410 (2013).
Google Scholar
Stern, N. et al. The structure of the lower pleistocene archaeological record: A case study From the Koobi Fora Formation [and Comments and Reply]. Curr. Anthropol. 34, 201–225 (1993).
Stern, N. The implications of time-averaging for reconstructing the land-use patterns of early tool-using hominids. J. Hum. Evol. 27, 89–105 (1994).
Blumenschine, R. J. et al. Environments and hominin activities across the FLK Peninsula during Zinjanthropus times (1.84 Ma), Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. J. Hum. Evol. 63, 364–383 (2012).
Google Scholar
Ferraro, J. V. et al. Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS ONE 8, e62174 (2013).
Google Scholar
Dibble, H. L. et al. Major fallacies surrounding stone artifacts and assemblages. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 24, 813–851 (2017).
Wilson, M. L. Long-term studies of the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In Long-Term Field Studies of Primates (eds Kappeler, P. M. & Watts, D. P.) 357–384 (Springer, 2012).
Proffitt, T., Haslam, M., Mercader, J. F., Boesch, C. & Luncz, L. V. Revisiting Panda 100, the first archaeological chimpanzee nut-cracking site. J. Hum. Evol. 124, 117–139 (2018).
Google Scholar
Panger, M. A., Brooks, A. S., Richmond, B. G. & Wood, B. Older than the Oldowan? Rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use. Evol. Anthropol. 11, 235–245 (2003).
Premo, L. Agent-based models as behavioral laboratories for evolutionary anthropological research. Ariz. Anthropol. 17, 91–113 (2006).
Premo, L. S. Exploratory agent-based models: Towards an experimental ethnoarchaeology. In Digital Discovery. Exploring New Frontiers in Human Heritage. CAA2006. Computer Applications and Quantitative Methods in Archaeology. Proceedings of the 34th Conference 22–29 (Archaeolingua, 2007).
Faith, J. T. et al. Rethinking the ecological drivers of hominin evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2021.04.011 (2021).
Google Scholar
Wurzer, G., Kowarik, K. & Reschreiter, H. Agent-Based Modeling and Simulation in Archaeology Vol. 7 (Springer, 2015).
Boesch, C. Wild cultures a comparison between chimpanzee and human cultures. (Cambridge University Press, 2014).
Masad, D. & Kazil, J. MESA: An agent-based modeling framework. Proceedings of the 14th Python in Science Conference (SCIPY 2015) 53–60 (2015).
Grimm, V. et al. The ODD protocol: A review and first update. Ecol. Model. 221, 2760–2768 (2010).
Koops, K., McGrew, W. C. & Matsuzawa, T. Ecology of culture: Do environmental factors influence foraging tool use in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus?. Anim. Behav. 85, 175–185 (2013).
Visalberghi, E., Sirianni, G., Fragaszy, D. & Boesch, C. Percussive tool use by Taï Western chimpanzees and Fazenda Boa Vista bearded capuchin monkeys: A comparison. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370, 20140351 (2015).
Google Scholar
Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399, 682–685 (1999).
Google Scholar
Potts, R. Variables versus models of early Pleistocene hominid land use. J. Hum. Evol. 27, 7–24 (1994).
Potts, R., Behrensmeyer, A. K. & Ditchfield, P. Paleolandscape variation and early Pleistocene hominid activities: Members 1 and 7, Olorgesailie formation, Kenya. J. Hum. Evol. 37, 747–788 (1999).
Google Scholar
Foley, R. A model of regional archaeological structure. Proc. Prehist. Soc 47, 1–17 (1981).
Google Scholar
Maurin, T., Bertran, P., Delagnes, A. & Boisserie, J.-R. Early hominin landscape use in the Lower Omo Valley, Ethiopia: Insights from the taphonomical analysis of Oldowan occurrences in the Shungura Formation (Member F). J. Hum. Evol. 111, 33–53 (2017).
Google Scholar
Binford, L. R. Constructing Frames of Reference (University of California Press, 2001).
Source: Ecology - nature.com