More stories

  • in

    Human encroachment into wildlife gut microbiomes

    1.Cunningham, A. A., Daszak, P. & Wood, J. L. N. One health, emerging infectious diseases and wildlife: two decades of progress? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372, 20160167 (2017).2.Suzan, G., Esponda, F., Carrasco-Hernández, R. & Aguirre, A. A. in New Directions in Conservation Medicine: Applied Cases of Ecological Health (eds. Aguirre, A. A., Ostfeld, R. & Daszak, P.). 135–150 (Oxford University Press USA, 2012).3.Hussain, S., Ram, M. S., Kumar, A., Shivaji, S. & Umapathy, G. Human presence increases parasitic load in endangered lion-tailed macaques (Macaca silenus) in its fragmented rainforest habitats in Southern India. PLoS ONE 8, 1–8 (2013).
    Google Scholar 
    4.Junge, R. E., Barrett, M. A. & Yoder, A. D. Effects of anthropogenic disturbance on indri (Indri indri) health in Madagascar. Am. J. Primatol. 73, 632–642 (2011).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    5.Friggens, M. M. & Beier, P. Anthropogenic disturbance and the risk of flea-borne disease transmission. Oecologia 164, 809–820 (2010).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    6.Woodroffe, R. et al. Contact with domestic dogs increases pathogen exposure in endangered African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). PLoS ONE 7, e30099 (2012).7.Crowl, T. A., Crist, T. O., Parmenter, R. R., Belovsky, G. & Lugo, A. E. The spread of invasive species and infectious disease as drivers of ecosystem change. Front. Ecol. Environ. 6, 238–246 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    8.Keesing, F., Holt, R. D. & Ostfeld, R. S. Effects of species diversity on disease risk. Ecol. Lett. 9, 485–498 (2006).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    9.Gámez-Virués, S. et al. Landscape simplification filters species traits and drives biotic homogenization. Nat. Commun. 6, 8568 (2015).10.Alberdi, A., Aizpurua, O., Bohmann, K., Zepeda-Mendoza, M. L. & Gilbert, M. T. P. Do vertebrate gut metagenomes confer rapid ecological adaptation? Trends Ecol. Evol. 31, 689–699 (2016).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    11.Hooper, L. V., Littman, D. R. & Macpherson, A. J. Interactions between the microbiota and the immune system. Science 336, 1268–1273 (2012).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    12.Shapira, M. Gut microbiotas and host evolution: scaling up symbiosis. Trends Ecol. Evol. 31, 539–549 (2016).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    13.Brugman, S. et al. A comparative review on microbiota manipulation: lessons from fish, plants, livestock, and human research. Front. Nutr. 5, 1–15 (2018).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    14.Wasimuddin et al. Astrovirus infections induce age-dependent dysbiosis in gut microbiomes of bats. ISME J. 12, 2883–2893 (2018).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    15.Wasimuddin et al. Adenovirus infection is associated with altered gut microbial communities in a non-human primate. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–12 (2019).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    16.Wilkins, L. J., Monga, M. & Miller, A. W. Defining dysbiosis for a cluster of chronic diseases. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–10 (2019).
    Google Scholar 
    17.Brüssow, H. Problems with the concept of gut microbiota dysbiosis. Microb. Biotechnol. 13, 423–434 (2020).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    18.Otto, S. P. Adaptation, speciation and extinction in the Anthropocene. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 285, 20182047 (2018).19.Amato, K. R. et al. Habitat degradation impacts black howler monkey (Alouatta pigra) gastrointestinal microbiomes. ISME J. 7, 1344–1353 (2013).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    20.Ingala, M. R., Becker, D. J., Bak Holm, J., Kristiansen, K. & Simmons, N. B. Habitat fragmentation is associated with dietary shifts and microbiota variability in common vampire bats. Ecol. Evol. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.5228 (2019)21.Juan, P. A. S., Hendershot, J. N., Daily, G. C. & Fukami, T. Land-use change has host-specificinfluenc on avian gut microbiomes. ISME J. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41396-019-0535-4 (2019)22.Barelli, C. et al. Habitat fragmentation is associated to gut microbiota diversity of an endangered primate: implications for conservation. Sci. Rep. 5, 14862 (2015).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    23.de Juan, S., Thrush, S. F. & Hewitt, J. E. Counting on β-diversity to safeguard the resilience of estuaries. PLoS ONE 8, 1–11 (2013).
    Google Scholar 
    24.Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach (Springer, 2002).25.Oksanen, J. et al. vegan: Community Ecology Package. R package version 2.5-5. https://github.com/vegandevs/vegan (2019).26.Nakagawa, S. & Cuthill, I. C. Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. 82, 591–605 (2007).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    27.Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988).28.Gillingham, M. A. F. et al. Offspring microbiomes differ across breeding sites in a panmictic species. Front. Microbiol. 10, 35 (2019).29.Anderson, M. J. Distance-based tests for homogeneity of multivariate dispersions. Biometrics 62, 245–253 (2006).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    30.Mandal, S. et al. Analysis of composition of microbiomes: a novel method for studying microbial composition. Microb. Ecol. Heal. Dis. 26, 1–7 (2015).
    Google Scholar 
    31.Douglas, G. M. et al. PICRUSt2 for prediction of metagenome functions. Nat. Biotechnol. 38, 669–673 (2020).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    32.Louca, S. & Doebeli, M. Efficient comparative phylogenetics on large trees. Bioinformatics 34, 1053–1055 (2018).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    33.Barbera, P. et al. EPA-ng: massively parallel evolutionary placement of genetic sequences. Syst. Biol. 68, 365–369 (2019).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    34.Czech, L., Barbera, P. & Stamatakis, A. Genesis and Gappa: processing, analyzing and visualizing phylogenetic (placement) data. Bioinformatics 36, 3263–3265 (2020).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    35.Nyhus, P. J. Human—wildlife conflict and coexistence. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 41, 143–171 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    36.Foden, W. B. et al. Climate change vulnerability assessment of species. WIREs Clim. Chang. 10, 1–36 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    37.Beck, J. M. et al. Multicenter comparison of lung and oral microbiomes of HIV-infected and HIV-uninfected individuals. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 192, 1335–1344 (2015).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    38.Pita, L., Rix, L., Slaby, B. M., Franke, A. & Hentschel, U. The sponge holobiont in a changing ocean: from microbes to ecosystems. Microbiome 6, 46 (2018).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    39.Rosado, P. M. et al. Marine probiotics: increasing coral resistance to bleaching through microbiome manipulation. ISME J. 13, 921–936 (2019).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    40.Wang, L. et al. Corals and their microbiomes are differentially affected by exposure to elevated nutrients and a natural thermal anomaly. Front. Mar. Sci. 5, 1–16 (2018).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    41.Zaneveld, J. R., McMinds, R. & Thurber, R. V. Stress and stability: applying the Anna Karenina principle to animal microbiomes. Nat. Microbiol. 2, 17121 (2017).42.Rocca, J. D. et al. The Microbiome Stress Project: toward a global meta-analysis of environmental stressors and their effects on microbial communities. Front. Microbiol. 10, 3272 (2019).43.Gillingham, M. A. F. et al. Bioaccumulation of trace elements affects chick body condition and gut microbiome in greater flamingos. Sci. Total Environ. 761, 143250 (2020).44.Chase, J. M. Stochastic community assembly causes higher biodiversity in more productive environments. Science 328, 1388–1392 (2010).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    45.Jiménez, R. R., Alvarado, G., Estrella, J. & Sommer, S. Moving beyond the host: unraveling the skin microbiome of endangered Costa Rican amphibians. Front. Microbiol. 10, 1–18 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    46.Wang, J. et al. Phylogenetic beta diversity in bacterial assemblages across ecosystems: deterministic versus stochastic processes. ISME J. 7, 1310–1321 (2013).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    47.Chase, J. M. & Myers, J. A. Disentangling the importance of ecological niches from stochastic processes across scales. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 2351–2363 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    48.Pound, K. L., Lawrence, G. B. & Passy, S. I. Beta diversity response to stress severity and heterogeneity in sensitive versus tolerant stream diatoms. Divers. Distrib. 25, 374–384 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    49.Zhou, J. & Ning, D. Stochastic Community Assembly: does it matter in microbial ecology? Microbiol. Mol. Biol. Rev. 81, 1–32 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    50.Nicholas, R. A. J. & Ayling, R. D. Mycoplasma bovis: disease, diagnosis, and control. Res. Vet. Sci. 74, 105–112 (2003).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    51.Ley, D. H. in Diseases of Poultry (eds. et al.) (Blackwell Publishing, 2008).52.Groebel, K., Hoelzle, K., Wittenbrink, M. M., Ziegler, U. & Hoelzle, L. E. Mycoplasma suis invades porcine erythrocytes. Infect. Immun. 77, 576–584 (2009).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    53.do Nascimento, N. C., Santos, A. P., Guimaraes, A. M. S., Sanmiguel, P. J. & Messick, J. B. Mycoplasma haemocanis—the canine hemoplasma and its feline counterpart in the genomic era. Vet. Res. 43, 66 (2012).54.Hardham, J. M. et al. Transfer of Bacteroides splanchnicus to Odoribacter gen. nov. as Odoribacter splanchnicus comb. nov., and description of Odoribacter denticanis sp. nov., isolated from the crevicular spaces of canine periodontitis patients. Int. J. Syst. Evol. Microbiol. 58, 103–109 (2008).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    55.Kaakoush, N. O. Insights into the role of Erysipelotrichaceae in the human host. Front. Cell. Infect. Microbiol. 5, 1–4 (2015).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    56.Ormerod, K. L. et al. Genomic characterization of the uncultured Bacteroidales family S24-7 inhabiting the guts of homeothermic animals. Microbiome 4, 1–17 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    57.Herrmann, E. et al. RNA-based stable isotope probing suggests Allobaculum spp. as particularly active glucose assimilators in a complex murine microbiota cultured in vitro. Biomed Res. Int. 2017, 1829685 (2017).58.Greetham, H. L. et al. Allobaculum stercoricanis gen. nov., sp. nov., isolated from canine feces. Anaerobe 10, 301–307 (2004).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    59.Silva, Y. P., Bernardi, A. & Frozza, R. L. The role of short-chain fatty acids from gut microbiota in gut-brain communication. Front. Endocrinol. 11, 1–14 (2020).60.Wiegel, J., Tanner, R. & Rainey, F. A. in The Prokaryotes: Volume 4: Bacteria: Firmicutes, Cyanobacteria (eds. Dworkin, M., Falkow, S., Rosenberg, E., Schleifer, K.-H. & Stackebrandt, E.) 654–678 (Springer US, 2006).61.Tamanai-Shacoori, Z. et al. Roseburia spp.: a marker of health? Future Microbiol 12, 157–170 (2017).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    62.Freier, T. A., Beitz, D. C., Li, L. & Hartman, P. A. Characterization of Eubacterium coprostanoligenes sp. nov., a Cholesterol-Reducing Anaerobe. Int. J. Syst. Bacteriol. 44, 137–142 (1994).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    63.Venegas, D. P. et al. Short chain fatty acids (SCFAs)-mediated gut epithelial and immune regulation and its relevance for inflammatory bowel diseases. Front. Immunol. 10, 277 (2019).64.MetaCyc. MetaCyc Pathway: pyrimidine deoxyribonucleotides biosynthesis from CTP. https://biocyc.org/META/NEW-IMAGE?type=PATHWAY&object=PWY-7210&show-citations=T (2020).65.Caspi, R. et al. The MetaCyc database of metabolic pathways and enzymes. Nucleic Acids Res. 46, D633–D639 (2018).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    66.MetaCyc. MetaCyc Pathway: poly(glycerol phosphate) wall teichoic acid biosynthesis. https://biocyc.org/META/NEW-IMAGE?type=PATHWAY&object=TEICHOICACID-PWY (2020).67.Brown, S., Santa Maria, J. P. & Walker, S. Wall teichoic acids of gram-positive bacteria. Annu. Rev. Microbiol. 67, 313–336 (2013).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    68.MetaCyc. MetaCyc Pathway: L-lysine biosynthesis II. https://metacyc.org/META/NEW-IMAGE?type=PATHWAY&object=PWY-2941 (2020).69.Hutton, C. A., Perugini, M. A. & Gerrard, J. A. Inhibition of lysine biosynthesis: an evolving antibiotic strategy. Mol. Biosyst. 3, 458–465 (2007).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    70.Wanner, S. et al. Wall teichoic acids mediate increased virulence in Staphylococcus aureus. Nat. Microbiol. 2, 1–12 (2017).
    Google Scholar 
    71.MetaCyc. MetaCyc Pathway: formaldehyde assimilation II (assimilatory RuMP Cycle). https://biocyc.org/META/NEW-IMAGE?type=PATHWAY&object=PWY-1861 (2020).72.Chen, N. H., Djoko, K. Y., Veyrier, F. J. & McEwan, A. G. Formaldehyde stress responses in bacterial pathogens. Front. Microbiol. 7, 1–17 (2016).
    Google Scholar 
    73.Tauseef, S. M., Premalatha, M., Abbasi, T. & Abbasi, S. A. Methane capture from livestock manure. J. Environ. Manag. 117, 187–207 (2013).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    74.Dale, V. H., Brown, S., Calderón, M. O., Montoya, A. S. & Martínez, R. E. Estimating baseline carbon emissions for the eastern Panama Canal watershed. Mitig. Adapt. Strateg. Glob. Chang 8, 323–348 (2003).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    75.Schmid, J. et al. Ecological drivers of Hepacivirus infection in a neotropical rodent inhabiting landscapes with various degrees of human environmental change. Oecologia https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-018-4210-7 (2018)76.Adler, G. H. & Beatty, R. P. Changing reproductive rates in a neotropical forest rodent, Proechimys semispinosus. J. Anim. Ecol. 66, 472 (1997).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    77.Adler, G. H. Fruit and seed exploitation by Central American spiny rats, Proechimys semispinosus. Stud. Neotrop. Fauna Environ. 30, 237–244 (1995).78.Hoch, G. A. & Adler, G. H. Removal of black palm (Astrocaryum standleyanum) seeds by spiny rats (Proechimys semispinosus). J. Trop. Ecol. 13, 51–58 (1997).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    79.Endries, M. J. & Adler, G. H. Spacing patterns of a tropical forest rodent, the spiny rat (Proechimys semispinosus), in Panama. J. Zool. 265, 147–155 (2005).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    80.Adler, G. H. The island syndrome in isolated populations of a tropical forest rodent. Oecologia 108, 694–700 (1996).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    81.Caporaso, J. G. et al. Global patterns of 16S rRNA diversity at a depth of millions of sequences per sample. PNAS 108, 4516–4522 (2011).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    82.Caporaso, J. G. et al. QIIME allows analysis of high-throughput community sequencing data. Nat. Methods 7, 335–336 (2010).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    83.Menke, S. et al. Oligotyping reveals differences between gut microbiomes of free-ranging sympatric Namibian carnivores (Acinonyx jubatus, Canis mesomelas) on a bacterial species-like level. Front. Microbiol. 5, 526 (2014).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    84.Bolyen, E. et al. Reproducible, interactive, scalable and extensible microbiome data science using QIIME 2. Nat. Biotechnol. 37, 852–857 (2019).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    85.Callahan, B. J. et al. DADA2: high-resolution sample inference from Illumina amplicon data. Nat. Methods 13, 581–583 (2016).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    86.Callahan, B. J., Mcmurdie, P. J. & Holmes, S. P. Exact sequence variants should replace operational taxonomic units in marker-gene data analysis. ISME J. 11, 2639–2643 (2017).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    87.Quast, C. et al. The SILVA ribosomal RNA gene database project: Improved data processing and web-based tools. Nucleic Acids Res. 41, 590–596 (2013).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    88.Yilmaz, P. et al. The SILVA and ‘All-species Living Tree Project (LTP)’ taxonomic frameworks. Nucleic Acids Res. 42, 643–648 (2014).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    89.Glöckner, F. O. et al. 25 years of serving the community with ribosomal RNA gene reference databases and tools. J. Biotechnol. 261, 169–176 (2017).PubMed 
    Article 
    CAS 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    90.Pedregosa, F. et al. Scikit-learn: machine learning in python. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 12, 2825–2830 (2011).91.Price, M. N., Dehal, P. S. & Arkin, A. P. FastTree 2—āpproximately maximum-likelihood trees for large alignments. PLoS ONE 5, e9490 (2010).92.Huson, D. H. & Scornavacca, C. Dendroscope 3: an interactive tool for rooted phylogenetic trees and networks. Syst. Biol. 61, 1061–1067 (2012).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    93.R. Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.r-project.org/index.html (2017).94.McMurdie, P. J. & Holmes, S. phyloseq: An R package for reproducible interactive analysis and graphics of microbiome census data. PLoS ONE 8, e61217 (2013).95.Davis, N. M., Proctor, Di. M., Holmes, S. P., Relman, D. A. & Callahan, B. J. Simple statistical identification and removal of contaminant sequences in marker-gene and metagenomics data. Microbiome 6, 1–14 (2018).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    96.Shannon, C. E. A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 27, 379–423 (1948).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    97.Faith, D. P. Conservation evaluation and phylogenetic diversity. Biol. Conserv. 61, 1–10 (1992).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    98.Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M. & Walker, S. C. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).99.Mcmurdie, P. J. & Holmes, S. Waste not, want not: why rarefying microbiome data is inadmissible. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003531 (2014).100.Kim, Y. S., Unno, T., Kim, B.-Y. & Park, M. Sex differences in gut microbiota. World J. Mens. Health 38, 48–60 (2020).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    101.Kolodny, O. et al. Coordinated change at the colony level in fruit bat fur microbiomes through time. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 3, 116–124 (2019).PubMed 
    Article 
    PubMed Central 

    Google Scholar 
    102.Kartzinel, T. R., Hsing, J. C., Musili, P. M., Brown, B. R. P. & Pringle, R. M. Covariation of diet and gut microbiome in African megafauna. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 23588–23593 (2019).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    103.Zuur, A., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N., Saveliev, A. A. & Smith, G. M. Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R (Springer Science & Business Media, 2009).104.Nakagawa, S. & Schielzeth, H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 133–142 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    105.Brooks, M. E. et al. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R. J. 9, 378–400 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    106.Lozupone, C. & Knight, R. UniFrac: a new phylogenetic method for comparing microbial communities. Appl. Environ. Microbiol. 71, 8228–8235 (2005).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    107.Lozupone, C. A., Hamady, M., Kelley, S. T. & Knight, R. Quantitative and qualitative β diversity measures lead to different insights into factors that structure microbial communities. Appl. Environ. Microbiol. 73, 1576–1585 (2007).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    108.Anderson, M. J. Permutational Multivariate Analysis of Variance (PERMANOVA). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118445112.stat07841. (2017)109.Anderson, M. J. & Walsh, D. C. I. PERMANOVA, ANOSIM, and the Mantel test in the face of heterogeneous dispersions: what null hypothesis are you testing? Ecol. Monogr. 83, 557–574 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    110.Li, H. et al. Pika population density is associated with the composition and diversity of gut microbiota. Front. Microbiol. 7, 1–9 (2016).
    Google Scholar 
    111.Weiss, S. et al. Normalization and microbial differential abundance strategies depend upon data characteristics. Microbiome 5, 1–18 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    112.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300 (1995).
    Google Scholar 
    113.Fackelmann, G. gfackelmann/human-encroachment-into-wildlife-gut-microbiomes: Release 1.0.0. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4725220. (2021) More

  • in

    Social transmission in the wild can reduce predation pressure on novel prey signals

    Study siteThe experiment was conducted at Madingley Wood, Cambridgeshire, UK (0◦3.2´E, 52◦12.9´N) during summer 2018. Madingley Wood is an established field site with an ongoing long-term study of the blue tit and great tit populations. During the autumn and winter birds are caught from feeding stations using mist nets and they are fitted with British Trust of Ornithology (BTO) ID rings. Since 2012, blue tits and great tits have been fitted with RFID tags (BTO Special Methods permit to HMR), which enables collecting data remotely about their foraging behavior and social relationships. The study site has 90 nest boxes that are monitored annually during the breeding season. In 2018 chicks (n = 325) fledged successfully from 45 nest boxes (blue tits = 21, great tits = 24) and they were all ringed and fitted with RFID tags when they were approximately 10 days old. Because new juvenile flocks were arriving at our study site throughout the summer, we also conducted several mist-netting and ringing sessions in July and August to maintain a high proportion of blue tits and great tits ringed and RFID tagged for the experiments (on average 89%, see below). The study protocol was approved by the Animal Users Committee at the Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge.Food itemsWe investigated birds’ foraging choices by offering them colored almond flakes at bird feeders that were distributed throughout the wood. Before beginning the experiments, we allowed the birds to become familiar with the food items by providing plain ‘control’ almonds (plain and not colored) in paired feeders (1.5 m apart) at three locations (approximately 170 m from each other). The feeders were surrounded with metal cages to exclude larger birds, and we placed plastic buckets under the feeders to collect any spilled almonds and minimize birds’ opportunities to forage from the ground. We introduced the feeders at the beginning of June when the nestlings had fledged and were beginning to forage independently, and continued to provide these plain almonds in between our learning experiments (Fig. 3c).In the learning experiments, almond flakes were dyed with non-toxic food dye (Classikool Concentrated Droplet Food Colouring). We used three different color pairs: green (Leaf Green) and red (Bright Red), purple (Lavender Purple) and blue (Royal Blue), and orange (Satsuma Orange) and yellow (Dandelion Yellow). Almond flakes were dyed by soaking them for approximately 20 min in a solution of 900 ml of water and 30 ml of food dye and then left to air dry for 48 h. In the avoidance learning experiments, we made half of the almond flakes unpalatable by soaking them for one hour in 67% solution of chloroquine phosphate, following previously established methods from avoidance learning studies with birds in captivity14,23,24,25. The food dye was added to the solution during the last 20 min.Red and green are common colors used by aposematic, or cryptic prey, respectively9. Therefore, we investigated whether blue tits and great tits had initial color biases towards these colors before starting the main experiment. Because we did not want the birds in our study population to have any experience of the colors before the main experiment, this pilot study was conducted in Newbury, which is 130 km from our main study site. Birds were simultaneously presented with two feeders containing red and green almonds (both palatable) for 30 min and the number of almonds of each color taken by blue tits or great tits was recorded using binoculars. The position of the feeders was switched after 15 min to control for any preferences for feeder location, and the test was repeated on 9 different days. We did not find any evidence that birds had initial color preferences (t-test: t = 0, df = 15.69, p = 1). For the other two learning experiments, we chose color pairs that were available as a food dye and as different from red and green in the visible spectrum as possible to avoid generalization across experiments. These color pairs (blue/purple and yellow/orange) had similar contrast ratios as green and red, based on their RGB values (measured from photographs, see Supplementary Information). Although avian and human vision is different, the discriminability of colors is likely to be similar51, and rapid avoidance learning in each experiment shows that all colors were easily distinguishable. This was the main requirement for testing social information use, and subtle differences in color pair discriminability should only introduce noise to our data but not influence our conclusions.Learning experiments with colored almondsWe conducted three avoidance learning experiments with different color pairs throughout the summer: red/green, blue/purple, and yellow/orange (unpalatable/palatable). In addition, we conducted a reversal-learning experiment with the blue/purple color pair by making both colors palatable after birds had acquired avoidance to blue almonds. Each experiment followed a similar protocol, in which birds were presented with colored almonds at the same three feeding stations where they were previously offered plain almonds. Each feeding station had two feeders, where one contained the palatable color and the other contained the unpalatable color (except in the reversal learning test when both colors were palatable). We switched the side of the feeders every day to make sure that birds learned to associate palatability with an almond color and not a feeder position. The feeders were filled at least once a day (or more often if necessary) to make sure that birds always had access to both colors. We continued each avoidance learning experiment until >90% of all recorded visits were to the feeder with palatable almonds, indicating that most birds in the population had learned to discriminate the colors. This took 7 days in the red/green experiment and 8 days in the other two color pairs (blue/purple and yellow/orange). The reversal learning experiment was finished after 9 days when 50% of the visits were to the previously unpalatable color (blue), indicating that most birds had reversed their learned avoidance towards it.Recording visits to feedersWe monitored visits to all feeders using RFID antennas and data loggers (Francis Scientific Instruments, Ltd) that scanned birds’ unique RFID tag codes when they landed on a perch attached to the feeder. During the learning experiments, each day we also recorded videos from all three feeding stations (using Go Pro Hero Action Camera and Canon Legria HF R66 Camcorder). From the videos, we monitored the proportion of blue tits and great tits that did not have RFID tags and were therefore not recorded when visiting the feeders. We calculated the estimated RFID tag coverage for each day of the experiments by watching at least 100 visits to the feeders from the videos (divided equally among the three feeding stations) and recording whether blue tits and great tits had an RFID tag or not. We realized that the number of untagged individuals was very high (approximately 50% of all visiting birds) when we started the experiment with the first color pair (red/green; see Supplementary Fig. 3). We, therefore, stopped the experiment after two days and caught birds from the feeding stations with mist nets to fit RFID tags to new individuals. To maintain a high number of individuals RFID tagged for the other color pairs, we conducted a mist netting session a day before starting each experiment, as well as 4–5 days after it. We always switched the feeders back to containing plain almonds during mist-netting sessions to ensure that this would not interfere with the learning experiments. Apart from the first two days of the red/green experiment, the RFID tag coverage was on average 89% throughout the experiments (varying between 80 and 95%, Supplementary Fig. 3).Birds were recorded every time that they visited the feeders, i.e., landed on the RFID antenna. However, it is possible that birds did not take the almond during every visit. To get an estimate of how often birds landed on the antenna without taking the almond, and whether this differed between palatable and unpalatable colors, we analyzed the visits to the feeders from the video recordings. We watched videos from the five first days of each experiment (i.e., different color pairs) and analyzed 60 visits to each color (divided approximately equally among the three feeding stations). We recorded whether the feeding event happened (birds ate the almond at the feeder or flew away with it) or whether birds left the feeder without sampling the almond. Because the number of visits to the unpalatable feeder was low during the last days of the avoidance learning experiments, we decided not to analyze avoidance learning videos after day five (but recorded visits from all days of the reversal learning experiment). We found that in avoidance learning experiments birds started to ‘reject’ unpalatable almonds after two days, i.e., they sometimes landed on the feeder but flew away without taking the almond (see Supplementary Fig. 4a). This change was not observed at palatable feeders where birds continued to consume almonds at a similar rate as at the beginning of the experiment (Supplementary Fig. 4a). In reversal learning, the proportion of visits that did not include a feeding event did not differ between purple and blue almonds: birds showed similar hesitation towards both colors at the beginning of the experiment, but this wariness decreased when the experiment progressed, with birds taking the almond during most of their visits (Supplementary Fig. 4b).Statistical analyses and model validationForaging choices in learning experimentsWe first analyzed how birds’ foraging choices changed during the learning experiments using generalized linear mixed-effects models with a binomial error distribution. The number of times an individual visited each feeder on each day of the experiment was used as a bounded response variable, and this was explained by species (blue tit/great tit), individuals’ age (juvenile/adult), and day of the experiment (continuous variable), as well as bird identity as a random effect. When analyzing avoidance learning, initial exploration of data suggested that results were similar across all three experiments, so we combined the experiments in the same model. To investigate whether learning curves differed between the species or age groups, the day of the experiment was included as a second-order polynomial term, and we started model selections with models that included a three-way interaction between species, age, and day2. Best-fitting models were selected based on Akaike’s information criterion (see Supplementary Tables 1 and 2).Social networkTo investigate if birds used social information in their foraging choices, we first constructed a social network of the bird population based on their visits to feeders outside of the learning experiments, i.e., when birds were presented with plain almonds (in total 92 days, see Supplementary Information for the robustness of analysis to exclusion of network data before or after the experiment). We used only these data as individuals were likely to vary in their hesitation to visit novel colored almonds. We used a Gaussian mixture model to detect the clusters of visits (‘gathering events’) at the feeders52 and then calculated association strengths between individuals based on how often they were observed in the same group (gambit of the group approach). These associations (network edges) were calculated using the simple ratio index, SRI35.$$frac{x}{x+{y}_{{mathrm{A}}}+{y}_{{mathrm{B}}}+{y}_{{{mathrm{AB}},}}}$$
    (2)
    where x is the number of samples where individuals A and B co-occurred in the same group, yA is the number of samples where only individual A was seen, yB is the number of samples where only individual B was seen, and yAB is the number of samples where both A and B were observed in the same sample but not together. Network associations, therefore, estimated the probability that two individuals were in the same group at a given time, with the values scaled between 0 (never observed in the same group) and 1 (always observed in the same group).Social information use during avoidance learning: model descriptionIf social avoidance learning was occurring, then the more birds observed negative responses of others feeding on the unpalatable feeder, the less likely they would be to choose the unpalatable feeder themselves. Thus, we expected the probability of bird j choosing the unpalatable option at time t to decrease with ({R}_{-,j}left(tright)) (the real number of negative feeding events observed by j prior to time t). Likewise, if appetitive social learning was occurring, then the more birds observed positive responses of others feeding on the palatable feeder, the more likely they would be to choose the palatable feeder themselves (rather than the unpalatable feeder). So, we also expected the probability of j choosing the unpalatable option at time t to decrease as ({R}_{+,j}left(tright),)(the real number of positive events observed by j prior to time t) increased.However, we could not test for an effect of ({R}_{-,j}left(tright)) and ({R}_{+,j}left(tright)) directly, since birds often ate the almond away from the feeder, and therefore the real number of observed feeding events could not be measured. Instead, we aimed to test for a pattern following the social network that is consistent with these social learning processes. We reasoned that the probability that one individual i, observes a specific feeding event by another individual j, was proportional to the network connection between them, aij (probability they are in the same feeding group at a given time). Therefore, in each avoidance learning experiment (i.e., different color pair), we calculated the expected number of negative feeding events observed, prior to each choice (occurring at time t) as$${O}_{-,i}left(tright)={sum }_{j}{N}_{-,j}left(tright){a}_{{ij}},$$
    (3)
    where ({N}_{-,j}left(tright)) was the number of times j had visited unpalatable almonds prior to time t (i ≠ j), and summation is across all birds in the network, and likewise for the expected number of positive feeding events:$${O}_{+,i}left(tright)={sum }_{j}{N}_{+,j}left(tright){a}_{{ij}},$$
    (4)
    where ({N}_{+,j}left(tright)) was the number of times j had visited palatable almonds prior to time t (i ≠ j).We analyzed whether the expected observations of positive and/or negative feeding events of others influenced the foraging choices in the avoidance learning experiments using generalized linear mixed-effects models with a binomial error distribution. We used each choice (i.e., visit a feeder) as a binary response variable (1 = unpalatable chosen, 0 = palatable chosen), with the probability that unpalatable feeder is chosen on feeding event E given by ({p}_{E}={p}_{-,{i}(E)}left({t}_{E}right)), where i(E) is the individual that fed during event E and ({t}_{E}) is the time at which event E occurred. We then modeled the probability of i choosing the unpalatable option at time t as:$${p}_{-,i}left(tright)={rm{logit}}left(alpha +{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}{N}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}{N}_{-,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}}+}{O}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{s{rm{oc}}-}{O}_{-,i}left(tright)+{{{{rm{B}}}}}_{i}right),$$
    (5)
    where ({N}_{+,i}left(tright)) is the number of times a choosing individual had visited the palatable feeder (positive personal information), ({N}_{-,i}left(tright)) is the number of times a choosing individual had visited the unpalatable feeder (negative personal information), ({O}_{+,i}left(tright)) is the expected number of observed positive (positive social information) and ({O}_{-,i}left(tright)) observed negative feeding events (negative social information). Bird identity was included as a random effect, ({{rm{{B}}}}_{i}) (age and species were later added as variables, see below). Parameters ({beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}) and ({beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}) are the effects of asocial learning about the palatable and unpalatable foods, ({beta }_{{rm{soc}}+}) is the effect of social learning about the palatable food, and ({beta }_{{rm{soc}}-})is the effect of social avoidance learning about the unpalatable food. Estimation of these parameters, with associated Wald tests and confidence intervals, allowed us to make inferences about which effects were operating and the size of these effects. To aid model fitting we standardized all predictor variables and then back-transformed the effects to the original scale (see Supplementary Tables 3–5 for the model outputs). To assess the importance of asocial and social effects, we also ran separate models that excluded either asocial or social parameters and compared them to the initial model in Eq. (5) using Akaike’s information criterion (see Supplementary Table 6). However, in most cases, this reduced model fit significantly, and we, therefore, kept all parameters in the final models.Our approach took ({O}_{-,{i}}left(tright)) as a measure of ({R}_{-,j}left(tright)), and ({O}_{+,{i}}left(tright)) as a measure of ({R}_{+,j}left(tright))-, which we termed the ‘expected’ number of observations of each type. Strictly speaking, ({O}_{-,{i}}left(tright)) and ({O}_{+,{i}}left(tright)) were upper limits on the expected number of observations, assuming that birds observed all feeding events in the groups in which they were present, whereas only an unknown proportion of such events (({p}_{o})) was observed. Therefore, the real expected number of negative/positive observations would be (Eleft({R}_{-,j}left(tright)right)={p}_{o}{O}_{-,{i}}left(tright)) and (Eleft({R}_{+,j}left(tright)right)={p}_{o}{O}_{+,{i}}left(tright)) respectively. Thus, the coefficient, ({beta }_{{rm{soc}}-}), for the effect of ({O}_{-,{i}}left(tright)) could be interpreted as ({beta }_{s{rm{oc}}-}={p}_{o}acute{{beta }_{{rm{soc}}-}}) where (acute{{beta }_{s{rm{oc}}-}}) is the effect per observation. Note that since ({p}_{o}le 1), and(,{beta }_{{rm{soc}}-}=acute{{beta }_{s{rm{oc}}-}}{p}_{o}), ({beta }_{s{rm{oc}}-}) is more likely to underestimate than overestimate the effect per observation of a negative feeding event. An analogous argument applies to the coefficient, ({beta }_{{rm{soc}}+}), for the effect of ({O}_{+,{i}}left(tright)).Social information use during avoidance learning: extension to test for species effectsAfter fitting the initial model shown in Eq. (5), we further broke down the model to test whether individuals were more likely to learn socially by observing conspecifics than heterospecifics. This was done by splitting the expected number of observed positive and negative feeding events to observations of conspecifics (({O}_{+{rm{C}},i}left(tright)), ({O}_{-{rm{C}},i}left(tright))) and heterospecifics (({O}_{+{rm{H}},i}left(tright)), ({O}_{-{rm{H}},i}left(tright))), and including these in the model as separate explanatory variables thus:$${p}_{-,i}(t)={rm{logit}}left(begin{array}{c}alpha +{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}{N}_{+,i}(t)+{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}{N}_{-,i}(t)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{H}}}{O}_{+{rm{H}},i}(t)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{H}}}{O}_{-{rm{H}},i}(t)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{C}}}{O}_{+{rm{C}},i}(t)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{C}}}{O}_{-{rm{C}},i}(t) \ +{{{{rm{B}}}}}_{i}end{array}right)$$
    (6a)
    with ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{H}}}) and ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{C}}}) giving the effect of a negative observation of a heterospecific and conspecific, respectively, whereas ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{H}}}) and ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{C}}}) give the effect of positive observation of a heterospecific and conspecific, respectively. In general –/+ subscripts refer to negative/positive feeding events and C/H subscripts to feeding events by conspecifics/heterospecifics. By re-parameterizing the model thus:$${p}_{-,i}left(tright)={rm{logit}}left(begin{array}{c}alpha +{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}{N}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}{N}_{-,i}left(tright) \ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{H}}+}{O}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{H}}-}{O}_{-,i}left(tright)\ +left({beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{C}}+}-{beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{H}}+}right){O}_{+{rm{C}},i}left(tright)+left({beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{C}}-}-{beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{H}}-}right){O}_{-{rm{C}},i}left(tright)\ +{{{{rm{B}}}}}_{i}end{array}right)$$
    (6b)
    we were able to test for a difference between observations of negative feeds by conspecifics and heterospecifics (left({beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{C}}-}-{beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{H}}-}right)) and between observations of positive feeds by conspecifics and heterospecifics (left({beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{C}}+}-{beta }_{{rm{soc}},{rm{H}}+}right)).For all experiments there was no evidence for a difference between ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{H}}}) and ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{C}}}) (yellow/orange: Z = 0.803, p = 0.42; red/green: Z = 0.065, p = 0.95; blue/purple: Z = 1.113, p = 0.27). However, there was some evidence of a difference between ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{H}}}) and ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{C}}}) in two of the three experiments (yellow/orange: Z = 1.359, p = 0.17; red/green: Z = 1.417, p = 0.16; blue/purple: Z = 0.729, p = 0.47). Consequently, we reduced the model down to:$${p}_{-,i}left(tright)={rm{logit}}left(begin{array}{c}alpha +{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}{N}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}{N}_{-,i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-}{O}_{-,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{H}}}{O}_{+{rm{H}},i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{C}}}{O}_{+{rm{C}},i}left(tright)\ +{{{{rm{B}}}}}_{i}end{array}right)$$
    (7)
    for further analysis, i.e., with different effects for observations of conspecific/heterospecific positive feeds, but not of negative feeds. We did this for all color combinations (including blue/purple) to allow comparison across experiments (see Table 1). The R code used to run these models can be found in Supplementary data53 in ‘GLMM models Orange Yellow final.r’.Social information use during avoidance learning: simulations to test for a network effectNext, we tested whether the social effects we detected followed the social network. When using a network-based diffusion analysis (NBDA43), researchers can compare a network model with one in which the network has homogeneous connections among all individuals, but we found this to be unreliable for our model. Instead, we used a simulation approach to generate a null distribution for the null hypothesis of homogeneous social effects, taking the size of the social effects from the fitted models. We ran 1000 simulations (using the same procedure described above) for all social effects that were found to be significant in each avoidance learning model (each color pair; see Table 1). The total number of expected observations was kept equal, but we homogenized the observation effect across all birds by replacing the probability of bird i observing a feed by bird j, previously ({a}_{{ij}}), with ({sum }_{i}{a}_{{ij}}/n), where n is the number of birds in the experiment, (i.e., all birds had the same probability of observing each feeding event). The model was fitted to the simulated data each time to extract the Z value (Wald test statistic) of the social effect of interest. The distribution of these values was then used as a null distribution to test whether our observed social effect differed from the effects that did not follow the social network. To this end, we calculated the proportion of simulations that yielded a Z value as extreme or more extreme than that observed (judged by distance in either direction from the mean of the null distribution). The R code used to run these simulations can be found in Supplementary data53 in ‘Simulations to test if network effects follow network Orange Yellow.r’.Social information use during avoidance learning: extension to test for age effectsWe then aimed to test whether each of the three social effects detected differed based on the age class of the observed individual (adult versus juveniles). We, therefore, split the negative expected observations ({O}_{-,i}left(tright)) into the expected observations of adults ({O}_{-{rm{A}},i}left(tright)) and juveniles ({O}_{-{rm{J}},i}left(tright)), each with its associated coefficient in the model ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{A}}}) and ({beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{J}}}). Likewise, we split positive observations of conspecifics as ({O}_{+{rm{CA}},i}left(tright)) and ({O}_{+{rm{CJ}},i}left(tright)) and positive observations of heterospecifics as ({O}_{+{rm{HA}},i}left(tright)) and ({O}_{+{rm{HJ}},i}left(tright)) to give the model:$${p}_{-,i}left(tright)={rm{logit}}left(begin{array}{c}alpha +{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}{N}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}{N}_{-,i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{A}}}{O}_{-{rm{A}},i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{J}}}{O}_{-{rm{J}},i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{HA}}}{O}_{+{rm{HA}},i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{HJ}}}{O}_{+{rm{HJ}},i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{CA}}}{O}_{+{rm{CA}},i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{CJ}}}{O}_{+{rm{CJ}},i}left(tright)\ +{{{{rm{B}}}}}_{i}end{array}right)$$
    (8a)
    As before, –/+ subscripts refer to negative/positive feeding events, C/H subscripts to feeding events by conspecifics/heterospecifics, and A/J subscripts to feeding events by adults/juveniles. We also fitted a re-parameterized version allowing us to test for a difference between expected observations of adults and observations of juveniles for each of the three social effects:$${p}_{-,i}left(tright)={rm{logit}}left(begin{array}{c}alpha +{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}+}{N}_{+,i}left(tright)+{beta }_{{rm{asoc}}-}{N}_{-,i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{J}}}{O}_{-,i}left(tright)+left({{beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{A}}}-beta }_{{rm{soc}},-{rm{J}}}right){O}_{-{rm{A}},i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{H}}}{O}_{+{rm{H}},i}left(tright)+left({{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{HA}}}-beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{HJ}}}right){O}_{+{rm{JA}},i}left(tright)\ +{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{C}}}{O}_{+{rm{C}},i}left(tright)+left({{beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{CA}}}-beta }_{{rm{soc}},+{rm{CJ}}}right){O}_{+{rm{CA}},i}left(tright)\ +{{{{rm{B}}}}}_{i}end{array}right)$$
    (8b)
    The R code used to run these models can be found in Supplementary data53 in ‘GLMM models Orange Yellow final.r’. The main results of each model are presented in Table 2 and full model outputs in Supplementary Tables 3–5.Social information use during reversal learningTo investigate social information use during reversal learning, we used the order of acquisition diffusion analysis (OADA), a variant of NBDA43, which explores the order in which individuals acquire a behavioral trait44. The rate of social transmission between two individuals is assumed to be linearly proportional to their network connection, and the spread of trait acquisition is therefore predicted to follow the network patterns if individuals are using social information. We used NBDA to investigate whether the order of individuals’ first visit to the previously unpalatable blue almonds (mimics) followed the network. We fitted several different models that included (i) only asocial learning, (ii) social transmission of information following a homogeneous network (equal associations among all individuals), or (iii) social transmission of information following our observed network. Models that included social transmission were further divided into models with equal or different transmission rates from adults and juveniles, and from conspecifics and heterospecifics, by constructing separate networks for each adult/juvenile and conspecific/heterospecific combination. To investigate whether asocial or social learning rates differed between blue tits and great tits, we included species as an individual-level variable. We then compared different social transmission models that assumed that species differed in both asocial and social learning rates, only in asocial or only in social learning rates, or that they did not differ in either (see Table 3). The best-supported model was selected using a model-averaging approach with Akaike’s information criterion corrected for small sample sizes. All analyses were conducted with the software R.3.6.154, using lme455, asnipe56, and NBDA57 packages.Reporting summaryFurther information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article. More

  • in

    Determinants of moult haulout phenology and duration in southern elephant seals

    1.IPCC Climate Change 2014: Synthesis report. In Contribution of Working Groups I, II and III to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (eds Core Writing Team et al.) (IPCC, 2014).
    Google Scholar 
    2.Keogan, K. et al. Global phenological insensitivity to shifting ocean temperatures among seabirds. Nat. Clim. Chang. 8, 313–318 (2018).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    3.Chambers, L. E. et al. Phenological changes in the southern hemisphere. PLoS ONE 8, E75514 (2013).ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    4.Thackeray, S. J. et al. Phenological sensitivity to climate across taxa and trophic levels. Nature 535, 241–245 (2016).ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    5.Forrest, J. & Miller-Rushing, A. J. Toward a synthetic understanding of the role of phenology in ecology and evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Series B Biol. Sci. 365, 3101–3112 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    6.Møller, A. P., Rubolini, D. & Lehikoinen, E. Populations of migratory bird species that did not show a phenological response to climate change are declining. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 16195–16200 (2008).ADS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    7.Hipfner, J. M. Matches and mismatches: Ocean climate, prey phenology and breeding success in a zooplanktivorous seabird. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 368, 295–304 (2008).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    8.Moyes, K. et al. Advancing breeding phenology in response to environmental change in a wild red deer population. Glob. Change Biol. 17, 2455–2469 (2011).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    9.Poloczanska, E. S. et al. Global imprint of climate change on marine life. Nat. Clim. Chang. 3, 919–925 (2013).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    10.Hauser, D. D. et al. Decadal shifts in autumn migration timing by Pacific Arctic beluga whales are related to delayed annual sea ice formation. Glob. Change Biol. 23, 2206–2217 (2017).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    11.Beltran, R. S., Kirkham, A. L., Breed, G. A., Ward Testa, J. & Burns, J. M. Reproductive success delays moult phenology in a polar mammal. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–12 (2019).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    12.Oosthuizen, W. C. et al. Individual heterogeneity in life-history trade-offs with age at first reproduction in capital breeding elephant seals. Popul. Ecol. 61, 421–435 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    13.Ling, J. K. The skin and hair of the southern elephant seal, Mirounga leonina (Linn). IV. Annual cycle of pelage follicle activity and moult. Aust. J. Zool. 60, 259–271 (2012).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    14.Johanos, T. C., Becker, B. L. & Ragen, T. J. Annual reproductive cycle of the female Hawaiian monk seal (Monachus schauinslandi). Mar. Mamm. Sci. 10, 13–30 (1994).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    15.Beltran, R. S., Burns, J. M. & Breed, G. A. Convergence of biannual moulting strategies across birds and mammals. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 285, 20180318 (2018).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    16.Boyd, I., Arnbom, T. & Fedak, M. Water flux, body composition, and metabolic rate during moult in female southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina). Physiol. Zool. 66, 43–60 (1993).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    17.Postma, M., Bester, M. N. & De Bruyn, P. J. N. Spatial variation in female southern elephant seal mass change assessed by an accurate non-invasive photogrammetry method. Antarct. Sci. 25, 731–740 (2013).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    18.Paterson, W. et al. Seals like it hot: Changes in surface temperature of harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) from late pregnancy to moult. J. Therm. Biol. 37, 454–461 (2012).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    19.Slip, D. J. & Burton, H. R. Population status and seasonal haulout patterns of the southern elephant seal (Mirounga leonina) at Heard Island. Antarct. Sci. 11, 38–47 (1999).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    20.Kirkman, S. et al. Variation in the timing of moult in southern elephant seals at Marion Island. S. Afr. J. Wildlife Res. 33, 79–84 (2003).
    Google Scholar 
    21.Ling, J. & Bryden, M. Southern elephant seal Mirounga leonina (Linnaeus), 1758. In Handbook of Marine Mammals Vol. 2 (eds Ridgway, S. H. & Harrison, R. J.) 297–327 (Academic Press, 1981).
    Google Scholar 
    22.Constable, A. J. et al. Climate change and southern ocean ecosystems I: how changes in physical habitats directly affect marine biota. Glob. Change Biol. 20, 3004–3025 (2014).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    23.Rogers, A. et al. Antarctic futures: An assessment of climate-driven changes in ecosystem structure, function, and service provisioning in the Southern Ocean. Ann. Rev. Mar. Sci. 12, 87–120 (2020).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    24.Hindell, M. A. & Burton, H. R. Seasonal haul-out patterns of the southern elephant seal (Mirounga leonina L.), at Macquarie Island. J. Mammal. 69, 81–88 (1988).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    25.Postma, M., Bester, M. N. & De Bruyn, P. J. N. Age-related reproductive variation in a wild marine mammal population. Polar Biol. 36, 719–729 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    26.Le Boeuf, B. J. & Laws, R. M. Elephant seals: An introduction to the genus. In Elephant Seals: Population Ecology, Behavior, and Physiology (eds Le Beouf, B. J. & Laws, R. M.) 1–28 (University Of California Press, 1994).Chapter 

    Google Scholar 
    27.De Bruyn, P. J. N. et al. Sex at sea: Alternative mating system in an extremely polygynous mammal. Anim. Behav. 82, 445–451 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    28.Oosthuizen, W. C., Pradel, R., Bester, M. N. & De Bruyn, P. J. N. Making use of multiple surveys: Estimating breeding probability using a multievent-robust design capture-recapture model. Ecol. Evolut. 9, 836–848 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    29.Bester, M. N. et al. The marine mammal programme at the Prince Edward Islands: 38 years of research. Afr. J. Mar. Sci. 33, 511–521 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    30.Pistorius, P. A., De Bruyn, P. J. N. & Bester, M. N. Population dynamics of southern elephant seals: A synthesis of three decades of demographic research at Marion Island. Afr. J. Mar. Sci. 33, 523–534 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    31.Pistorius, P. A., Bester, M. N. & Kirkman, S. P. Survivorship of a declining population of southern elephant seals, Mirounga leonina, in relation to age, sex and cohort. Oecologia 121, 201–211 (1999).ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    32.Oosthuizen, W. C., Bester, M. N., Altwegg, R., McIntyre, T. & De Bruyn, P. J. N. Decomposing the variance in southern elephant seal weaning mass: Partitioning environmental signals and maternal effects. Ecosphere 6, 139 (2015).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    33.Daniel, R. G., Jemison, L. A., Pendleton, G. W. & Crowley, S. M. Molting phenology of harbor seals on Tugidak Island, Alaska. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 19, 128–140 (2003).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    34.Chaise, L. L. et al. Local weather and body condition influence habitat use and movements on land of molting female southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina). Ecol. Evol. 8, 6081–6090 (2018).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    35.Dingemanse, N. J. & Dochtermann, N. A. Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: Mixed-effect modelling approaches. J. Anim. Ecol. 82, 39–54 (2013).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    36.Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R. & Huyvaert, K. P. AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: Some background, observations, and comparisons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 23–35 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    37.Zuur, A., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N., Saveliev, A. A. & Smith, G. M. Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R (Springer Science & Business Media, 2009).MATH 
    Book 

    Google Scholar 
    38.Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    39.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 2020). https://www.R-project.org/.40.Nakagawa, S. & Schielzeth, H. Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: A practical guide for biologists. Biol. Rev. 85, 935–956 (2010).PubMed 

    Google Scholar 
    41.Nakagawa, S. & Schielzeth, H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 133–142 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    42.Grosbois, V. et al. Assessing the impact of climate variation on survival in vertebrate populations. Biol. Rev. 83, 357–399 (2008).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    43.Forcada, J., Trathan, P. N. & Murphy, E. J. Life history buffering in Antarctic mammals and birds against changing patterns of climate and environmental variation. Glob. Change Biol. 14, 2473–2488 (2008).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    44.Bradshaw, C. J., Hindell, M. A., Sumner, M. D. & Michael, K. J. Loyalty pays: Potential life-history consequences of fidelity to marine foraging regions by southern elephant seals. Anim. Behav. 68, 1349–1360 (2004).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    45.Cotté, C., Park, Y.-H., Guinet, C. & Bost, C.-A. Movements of foraging king penguins through marine mesoscale eddies. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 274, 2385–2391 (2007).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    46.Barbraud, C. & Weimerskirch, H. Antarctic birds breed later in response to climate change. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103, 6248–6251 (2006).ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    47.Hindell, M. A. et al. Long-term breeding phenology shift in royal penguins. Ecol. Evol. 2, 1563–1571 (2012).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    48.Zimova, M. et al. Function and underlying mechanisms of seasonal colour moulting in mammals and birds: What keeps them changing in a warming world?. Biol. Rev. 93, 1478–1498 (2018).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    49.Thompson, P. & Rothery, P. Age and sex differences in the timing of moult in the common seal, Phoca vitulina. J. Zool. 212, 597–603 (1987).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    50.Cronin, M., Gregory, S. & Rogan, E. Moulting phenology of the harbour seal in south-west Ireland. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK 94, 1079–1086 (2014).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    51.Kirkman, S. et al. Variation in the timing of the breeding haulout of female southern elephant seals at Marion Island. Aust. J. Zool. 52, 379–388 (2004).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    52.Hindell, M. A., Slip, D. J. & Burton, H. R. Body mass loss of moulting female southern elephant seals, Mirounga leonina, at Macquarie Island. Polar Biol. 14, 275–278 (1994).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    53.Carlini, A., Marquez, M., Daneri, G. & Poljak, S. Mass changes during their annual cycle in females of southern elephant seals at King George Island. Polar Biol. 21, 234–239 (1999).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    54.Worthy, G. A. J., Morris, P. A., Costa, D. P. & Le Boeuf, B. J. Moult energetics of the northern elephant seal (Mirounga angustirostris). J. Zool. 227, 257–265 (1992).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    55.Arnbom, T., Fedak, M. & Rothery, P. Offspring sex ratio in relation to female size in southern elephant seals, Mirounga leonina. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 35, 373–378 (1994).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    56.Proffitt, K. M., Garrott, R. A., Rotella, J. J. & Wheatley, K. E. Environmental and senescent related variations in Weddell seal body mass: Implications for age-specific reproductive performance. Oikos 116, 1683–1690 (2007).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    57.Bérubé, C. H., Festa-Bianchet, M. & Jorgenson, J. T. Individual differences, longevity, and reproductive senescence in bighorn ewes. Ecology 80, 2555–2565 (1999).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    58.Oosthuizen, W. C., Péron, G., Pradel, R., Bester, M. N. & De Bruyn, P. J. N. Positive early-late life-history trait correlations in elephant seals. Ecology 102, e03288. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecy.3288 (2021).59.Kirby, V. L. & Ortiz, C. L. Hormones and fuel regulation in fasting elephant seals. In Elephant Seals: Population Ecology, Behavior, and Physiology (eds Le Beouf, B. J. & Laws, R. M.) 374–386 (University of California Press, 1994).Chapter 

    Google Scholar 
    60.Yochem, P. K. & Stewart, B. S. Hair and fur. In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals 3rd edn (eds Würsig, B. et al.) 447–448 (Academic Press, 2018).Chapter 

    Google Scholar 
    61.Ashwell-Erickson, S., Fay, F., Elsner, R. & Wartzok, D. Metabolic correlates of molting and regeneration of pelage in Alaskan harbour and spotted seals (Phoca vitulina and Phoca largha). Can. J. Zool. 64, 1086–1092 (1986).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    62.Laws, R. M. & Sinha, A. A. Reproduction. In Antarctic Seals: Research Methods and Techniques (ed. Laws, R. M.) 228–267 (Cambridge University Press, 1993).Chapter 

    Google Scholar 
    63.Smout, S., King, R. & Pomeroy, P. Environment-sensitive mass changes influence breeding frequency in a capital breeding marine top predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 89, 384–396 (2020).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    64.Clutton-Brock, T. & Sheldon, B. C. Individuals and populations: the role of long-term, individual-based studies of animals in ecology and evolutionary biology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 562–573 (2010).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    65.Cordes, L. S. & Thompson, P. M. Variation in breeding phenology provides insights into drivers of long-term population change in harbour seals. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 280, 20130847 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    66.Barbraud, C. et al. Contrasted demographic responses facing future climate change in Southern Ocean seabirds. J. Anim. Ecol. 80, 89–100 (2011).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    67.Atkinson, S., DeMaster, D. P. & Calkins, D. G. Anthropogenic causes of the western Steller sea lion Eumetopias jubatus population decline and their threat to recovery. Mamm. Rev. 38, 1–18 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    68.Desprez, M., McMahon, C. R., Hindell, M. A., Harcourt, R. & Gimenez, O. Known unknowns in an imperfect world: incorporating uncertainty in recruitment estimates using multi-event capture-recapture models. Ecol. Evol. 3, 4658–4668 (2013).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    69.Griffen, B. D. Reproductive skipping as an optimal life history strategy in the southern elephant seal, Mirounga leonina. Ecol. Evol. 8, 9158–9170 (2018).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    70.Rohwer, S., Viggiano, A. & Marzluff, J. M. Reciprocal tradeoffs between moult and breeding in albatrosses. Condor 113, 61–70 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    71.Grissot, A., Graham, I. M., Quinn, L., Bråthen, V. S. & Thompson, P. M. Breeding status influences timing but not duration of moult in the northern fulmar Fulmarus glacialis. Ibis 162, 446–459 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    72.Condit, R., Beltran, R.S., Robinson, P.W., Crocker, D.E. & Costa, D.P. Birth timing after the long feeding migration in elephant seals. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.10.02.324210 (in review). More

  • in

    Genetic diversity, population structure and historical demography of the two-spined yellowtail stargazer (Uranoscopus cognatus)

    1.Cámara, A. & Santero-Sánchez, R. Economic, social, and environmental impact of a sustainable fishereis model in Spain. Sustainability 11, 6311 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    2.Department of Fisheries Malaysia. Annual Fisheries statistics 2010–2019. https://www.dof.gov.my/index.php/pages/view/82 (2020).3.Department of Fisheries, Thailand. The annual marine fisheries statistics (2010–2019) based on the sample survey. https://elibonline.fisheries.go.th/elib/cgi-bin/opacexe.exe?op=dsp&bid=10498&lang=0&db=Main&pat=&cat=sub&skin=s&lpp=20&catop=edit&scid=zzz (2020).4.Jha, S., Deepti, V., Ravali, V. & Sujatha, K. Studies on some aspects of biology of Uranoscopus cognatus Cantor, 1849 (Pisces: Uranoscopidae) off Visakhapatnam, central eastern coast of India. Indian J. Mar. Sci. 48, 85–92 (2019).
    Google Scholar 
    5.Clark, M. R. et al. The impacts of deep-sea fisheries on benthic communities: A review. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 73(1), 51–69 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    6.Van Denderen, P. D. et al. Evaluating impacts of bottom trawling and hypoxia on benthic communities at the local, habitat, and regional scale using a modelling approach. ICES J. Mar. Sci. 77(1), 578–589 (2019).
    Google Scholar 
    7.Erdoğan Sağlam, N. & Sağlam, C. Population parameters of stargazer (Uranoscopus scaber Linnaeus, 1758) in the southeastern Black Sea region during the 2011–2012 fishing season. J. Appl. Ichthyol. 29, 1313–1317 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    8.Matsunuma, M. et al. Fishes of Terengganu: East Coast of Malay Peninsula, Malaysia (National Museum of Nature and Science, 2011).
    Google Scholar 
    9.Vilasri, V. Family Uranoscopidae. In Fishes of Southern Taiwan (eds Koeda, K. & Ho, H. S.) 1097–1105 (National Museum of Marine Biology & Aquarium, 2019).
    Google Scholar 
    10.Starks, E. C. The Osteology and Relationships of the Uranoscopoid Fishes (Stanford University Press, 1923).
    Google Scholar 
    11.Pietsch, T. W. Phylogenetic relationships of trachinoid fishes of the family Uranoscopidae. Copeia 1989, 253–303 (1989).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    12.Kishimoto, H. Uranoscopidae. In FAO Species Identification Guide for Fisheries Purposes (eds Carpenter, K. E. & Niem, V. H.) 3519–3531 (FAO, 2001).
    Google Scholar 
    13.Randall, J. E. & Arnold, R. J. Uranoscopus rosette, a new species of stargazer (Uranoscopidae: Trachinoidei) from the Red Sea. Aqua. Int. J. Ichthyol. 18, 209–219 (2012).
    Google Scholar 
    14.Jung-chen, H. & Hin-Kiu, M. Stargazers (Uranoscopidae) have exceptionally more bile. Kuroshio Sci. 9–1, 17–26 (2015).
    Google Scholar 
    15.Vilasri, V. Comparative anatomy and phylogenetic systematics of the family Uranoscopidae (Actinopterygii: Perciformes). Mem. Fac. Fish. Hokkaido Univ. 55, 1–106 (2013).
    Google Scholar 
    16.Fricke, R., Eschmeyer, W. N. & Van der Laan, R. (eds). Eschmeyer’s catalog of fishes: genera, species, references. http://researcharchive.calacademy.org/research/ichthyology/catalog/fishcatmain.asp (2020).17.Froese, R. & Pauly, D. Uranoscopidae. Fishbase https://www.fishbase.se/Summary/FamilySummary.php?ID=378 (2019).18.Fricke, R. Two new species of stargazers of the genus Uranoscopus (Teleostei: Uranoscopidae) from the western Pacific Ocean. Zootaxa 4476, 157–167 (2018).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    19.Fricke, R., Jawad, L. A., Al-Kharusi, L. H. & Al-Mamry, J. M. New record and redescription of Uranoscopus crassiceps Alcock, 1890 (Uranoscopidae) From Oman, Arabian Sea, Northwestern Indian Ocean, based on adult specimens. Cybium 37, 143–147 (2013).
    Google Scholar 
    20.Department of Fisheries Malaysia. Valid Local Name of Malaysian Marine Fishes (Department of Fisheries Malaysia, 2009).
    Google Scholar 
    21.Spalding, M. D. et al. Marine ecoregions of the world: A bioregionalization of coastal and shelf areas. Bioscience 57, 573–583 (2007).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    22.Voris, H. K. Maps of Pleistocene sea levels in Southeast Asia: Shorelines, river systems and time durations. J. Biogeogr. 27, 1153–1167 (2000).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    23.Rohfritsch, A. & Borsa, P. Genetic structure of Indian scad mackerel Decapterus russelli: Pleistocene vicariance and secondary contact in the Central Indo-West Pacific Seas. Heredity 95, 315 (2005).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    24.Lohman, D. J. et al. Biogeography of the Indo-Australian archipelago. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 42, 205–226 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    25.Crandall, E. D. et al. The molecular biogeography of the Indo-Pacific: Testing hypotheses with multispecies genetic patterns. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 28, 943–960 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    26.Reece, J. S., Bowen, B. W., Joshi, K., Goz, V. & Larson, A. Phylogeography of two moray eels indicates high dispersal throughout the Indo-Pacific. J. Hered. 101, 391–402 (2010).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    27.Akib, N. A. M. et al. High connectivity in Rastrelliger kanagurta: influence of historical signatures and migratory behaviour inferred from mtDNA cytochrome b. PLoS ONE 10, e0119749 (2015).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    28.Jamaludin, N. A. et al. Phylogeography of the Japanese scad, Decapterus maruadsi (Teleostei; Carangidae) across the Central Indo-West Pacific: evidence of strong regional structure and cryptic diversity. Mitochondrial DNA A 2, 1–13 (2020).
    Google Scholar 
    29.Gaither, M. R., Toonen, R. J., Robertson, D. R., Planes, S. & Bowen, B. W. Genetic evaluation of marine biogeographical barriers: perspectives from two widespread Indo-Pacific snappers (Lutjanus kasmira and Lutjanus fulvus). J. Biogeogr. 37, 133–147 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    30.Gaither, M. R. et al. Phylogeography of the reef fish Cephalopholis argus (Epinephelidae) indicates Pleistocene isolation across the Indo-Pacific Barrier with contemporary overlap in the Coral Triangle. BMC Evol. Biol. 11, 189 (2011).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    31.Timm, J. & Kochzius, M. Geological history and oceanography of the Indo-Malay Archipelago shape the genetic population structure in the false clown anemonefish (Amphiprion ocellaris). Mol. Ecol. 17, 3999–4014 (2008).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    32.Otwoma, L. M. & Kochzius, M. Genetic population structure of the coral reef sea star Linckia laevigata in the Western Indian Ocean and Indo-West Pacific. PLoS ONE 11, 10 (2016).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    33.Williams, S. T., Jara, J., Gomez, E. & Knowlton, N. The marine Indo-West Pacific break: Contrasting the resolving power of mitochondrial and nuclear genes. Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 941–952 (2002).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    34.Supmee, V., Sangthong, P., Songrak, A. & Suppapan, J. Population genetic structure of Asiatic Hard Clam (Meretrix meretrix) in Thailand based on Cytochrome Oxidase subunit I gene sequence. Biodiversitas 21, 2702–2709 (2020).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    35.Hui, M. et al. Comparative genetic population structure of three endangered giant clams (Cardiidae: Tridacna species) throughout the Indo-West Pacific: Implications for divergence, connectivity and conservation. J. Molluscan Stud. 82, 403–414 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    36.Panithanarak, T., Karuwancharoen, R., Na-Nakorn, U. & Nguyen, T. T. Population genetics of the spotted seahorse (Hippocampus kuda) in Thai waters: Implications for conservation. Zool. Stud. 49, 564–576 (2010).CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    37.Kasim, N. S. et al. Recent population expansion of longtail tuna Thunnus tonggol (Bleeker, 1851) inferred from the mitochondrial DNA markers. PeerJ 8, 9679 (2020).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    38.Canales-Aguirre, C. B., Ferrada-Fuentes, S., Galleguillos, R., Oyarzun, F. X. & Hernández, C. E. Population genetic structure of Patagonian toothfish (Dissostichus eleginoides) in the Southeast Pacific and Southwest Atlantic Ocean. PeerJ 6, e4173 (2018).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    39.Sato, M. et al. Genetic structure and demographic connectivity of marbled flounder (Pseudopleuronectes yokohamae) populations of Tokyo Bay. J. Sea Res. 142, 79–90 (2018).ADS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    40.Borsa, P. Genetic structure of round scad mackerel Decapterus macrosoma (Carangidae) in the Indo-Malay archipelago. Mar. Biol. 142, 575–581 (2003).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    41.Eytan, R. I. & Hellberg, M. E. Nuclear and mitochondrial sequence data reveal and conceal different demographic histories and population genetic processes in Caribbean reef fishes. Evolution 64, 3380–3397 (2010).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    42.Tan, M. P., Jamsari, A. F. J. & Siti Azizah, M. N. Genotyping of microsatellite markers to study genetic structure of the wild striped snakehead Channa striata in Malaysia. J. Fish. Biol. 88, 1932–1948 (2016).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    43.Tan, M. P., Jamsari, A. F. J. & Siti Azizah, M. N. Phylogeographic pattern of the striped snakehead, Channa striata in Sundaland: Ancient river connectivity, geographical and anthropogenic signatures. PLoS ONE 7, 1–11 (2012).
    Google Scholar 
    44.Tan, M. P., Jamsari, A. F. J., Muhlisin, Z. A. & Siti Azizah, M. N. Mitochondrial genetic variation and population structure of the striped snakehead, Channa striata in Malaysia and Sumatra. Indonesia. Biochem. Syst. Ecol. 60, 99–105 (2015).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    45.Haponski, A. E. & Stepien, C. A. Phylogenetic and biogeographical relationships of the Sander pikeperches (Percidae: Perciformes): Patterns across North America and Eurasia. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 110, 156–179 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    46.Milá, B., Van Tassell, J. L., Calderón, J. A., Rüber, L. & Zardoya, R. Cryptic lineage divergence in marine environments: Genetic differentiation at multiple spatial and temporal scales in the widespread intertidal goby Gobiosoma bosc. Ecol. Evol. 7, 5514–5523 (2017).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    47.Piganeau, G., Gardner, M. & Eyre-Walker, A. A broad survey of recombination in animal mitochondria. Mol. Biol. Evol. 21, 2319–2325 (2004).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    48.Avise, J. C. Molecular Markers, Natural History, and Evolution (Sinauer Associates Inc, 2004).
    Google Scholar 
    49.De Mandal, S., Chhakchhuak, L., Gurusubramanian, G. & Kumar, N. S. Mitochondrial markers for identification and phylogenetic studies in insects–A Review. DNA Barcodes 2, 1–9 (2014).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    50.Simon, C. et al. Evolution, weighting, and phylogenetic utility of mitochondrial gene sequences and a compilation of conserved polymerase chain reaction primers. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 87, 651–701 (1994).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    51.Hoofer, S. R., Reeder, S. A., Hansen, E. W. & Van Den Bussche, R. A. Molecular phylogenetics and taxonomic review of noctilionoid and vespertilionoid bats (Chiroptera: Yangochiroptera). J. Mammal. 84, 809–821 (2003).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    52.Hewitt, G. M. Speciation, hybrid zones and phylogeography or seeing genes in space and time. Mol. Ecol. 10, 537–549 (2001).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    53.Surya, S. et al. Morphometry and length-weight relationship of Uranoscopus marmoratus Cuvier, 1829 (Family: Uranoscopidae) from Palk Bay, India. Int. J. Biol. Sci. 5, 1–10 (2016).ADS 

    Google Scholar 
    54.Narejo, N. T. Morphometric characters and their relationships in Gudusia chapra (Hamilton) from Keenjhar Lake (Distt: Thatta), Sindh, Pakistan. Pak. J. Zool. 42, 101–104 (2010).
    Google Scholar 
    55.Gan, H. M., Nur Ilham Syahadah, M. Y., Vilasri, V., Tun Nurul Aimi, M. J. & Tan, M. P. Four whole mitogenome sequences of yellowtail stargazers (Uranoscopus cognatus cantor 1849) from East Peninsular Malaysia and West Coast of Thailand. Mitochondrial DNA B 4, 256–258 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    56.Panjarat, S. Sustainable fisheries in the Andaman Sea coast of Thailand. Division for Ocean Affairs and the Law of the Sea Office of Legal Affairs. (The United Nations, 2008).57.Derrick, B., Noranarttragoon, P., Zeller, D., Teh, L. C. & Pauly, D. Thailand’s missing marine fisheries catch (1950–2014). Front. Mar. Sci. 4, 402 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    58.Sampantamit, T., Ho, L., Van Echelpoel, W., Lachat, C. & Goethals, P. Links and trade-offs between fisheries and environmental protection in relation to the sustainable development goals in Thailand. Water 12, 399 (2020).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    59.Chong, V., Lee, P. & Lau, C. Diversity, extinction risk and conservation of Malaysian fishes. J. Fish Biol. 76, 2009–2066 (2010).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    60.Lim, H. C., Ahmad, A. T., Nuruddin, A. A. & Mohd Nor, S. A. Cytochrome b gene reveals panmixia among Japanese Threadfin Bream, Nemipterus japonicus (Bloch, 1791) populations along the coasts of Peninsular Malaysia and provides evidence of a cryptic species. Mitochondrial DNA A 27, 575–584 (2016).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    61.Nabilsyafiq, M. H. et al. ND5 gene marker reveals recent population expansion of wild pearse’s mudskipper (Periophthalmus novemradiatus Hamilton) inhabits Setiu wetlands in east Peninsular Malaysia. Malays. Appl. Biol. 48, 87–93 (2019).
    Google Scholar 
    62.Zhou, Y. et al. Importance of incomplete lineage sorting and introgression in the origin of shared genetic variation between two closely related pines with overlapping distributions. Heredity 118, 211–220 (2017).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    63.Lessios, H. A. The great American schism: divergence of marine organisms after the rise of the Central American Isthmus. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 39, 63–91 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    64.Avise, J. Molecular Markers, Natural History and Evolution (Chapman and Hall, 1994).Book 

    Google Scholar 
    65.Nelson, J. S., Grande, T. C. & Wilson, M. V. Fishes of the World (John Wiley and Sons, 2016).Book 

    Google Scholar 
    66.Young, J. Z. Memoirs: On the autonomic nervous system of the Teleostean Fish Uranoscopus scaber. J. Cell Sci. 2, 491–536 (1931).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    67.Day, J., Clark, J. A., Williamson, J. E., Brown, C. & Gillings, M. Population genetic analyses reveal female reproductive philopatry in the oviparous Port Jackson shark. Mar. Freshw. Res. 70, 986–994 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    68.Roycroft, E. J., Le Port, A. & Lavery, S. D. Population structure and male-biased dispersal in the short-tail stingray Bathytoshia brevicaudata (Myliobatoidei: Dasyatidae). Conserv. Genet. 20, 717–728 (2019).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    69.King, T. L., Eackles, M. S., Spidle, A. P. & Brockmann, H. J. Regional differentiation and sex-biased dispersal among populations of the horseshoe crab Limulus polyphemus. Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 134, 441–465 (2005).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    70.Lane, A. & Shine, R. Intraspecific variation in the direction and degree of sex-biased dispersal among sea-snake populations. Mol. Ecol. 20, 1870–1876 (2011).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    71.Casale, P., Laurent, L., Gerosa, G. & Argano, R. Molecular evidence of male-biased dispersal in loggerhead turtle juveniles. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 267, 139–145 (2002).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    72.Wyrtki, K. Physical Oceanography of the Southeast Asian Waters (University of California, 1961).
    Google Scholar 
    73.Barber, P., Palumbi, S., Erdmann, M. & Moosa, M. Sharp genetic breaks among populations of Haptosquilla pulchella (Stomatopoda) indicate limits to larval transport: patterns, causes, and consequences. Mol. Ecol. 11, 659–674 (2002).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    74.Kamarudin, K. R. & Esa, Y. Phylogeny and phylogeography of Barbonymus schwanenfeldii (Cyprinidae) from Malaysia inferred using partial cytochrome b mtDNA gene. J. Trop. Biol. Conserv. 5, 1–13 (2009).
    Google Scholar 
    75.Tan, M. P. et al. Genetic diversity of the Pearse’s Mudskipper Periophthalmus novemradiatus (Perciformes: Gobiidae) and characterization of its complete mitochondrial genome. Thalassas 36, 103–113 (2020).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    76.Roberts, T. R. & Khaironizam, M. Z. Trophic polymorphism in the Malaysian fish Neolissochilus soroides and other old world barbs (Teleostei, Cyprinidae). Nat. Hist. Bull. Siam Soc. 56, 25–53 (2008).
    Google Scholar 
    77.Forsman, A. Rethinking phenotypic plasticity and its consequences for individuals, populations and species. Heredity 115, 276 (2015).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    78.Vasileva, E. Morphokaryological variability and divergence of stargazers (Uranoscopus, perciformes) from the Mediterranean Sea basin: I. Divergence and taxonomic state of the Black Sea Stargazer. J. Ichthyol. 52, 476–484 (2012).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    79.Aljanabi, S. M. & Martinez, I. Universal and rapid salt-extraction of high quality genomic DNA for PCR-based techniques. Nucleic Acids Ress 25, 4692–4693 (1997).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    80.Ward, R. D., Zemlak, T. S., Innes, B. H., Last, P. R. & Hebert, P. D. DNA barcoding Australia’s fish species. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B Biol. Sci. 360, 1847–1857 (2005).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    81.López, J. A., Chen, W. J. & Ortí, G. Esociform phylogeny. Copeia 2004, 449–464 (2004).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    82.Mat Jaafar, T. N., Taylor, M. I., Mohd Nor, S. A., Bruyn, M. D. & Carvalho, G. R. Comparative genetic stock structure in three species of commercially exploited Indo-Malay Carangidae (Teleosteii, Perciformes). J. Fish Biol. 96, 337–349 (2020).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    83.Tamura, K., Stecher, G., Peterson, D., Filipski, A. & Kumar, S. MEGA6: molecular evolutionary genetics analysis version 6.0. Mol. Biol. Evol. 30, 2725–2729 (2013).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    84.Farris, J. S., Källersjö, M., Kluge, A. G. & Bult, C. Testing significance of incongruence. Cladistics 10, 315–319 (1994).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    85.Swofford, D. L. PAUP: Phylogenetic Analysis Using Parsimony (and Other Methods), Version 4.0 Beta 10 (Sinauer Associates, 2002).
    Google Scholar 
    86.Librado, P. & Rozas, J. DnaSP v5: A software for comprehensive analysis of DNA polymorphism data. Bioinformatics 25, 1451–1452 (2009).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    87.Excoffier, L. & Lischer, H. E. L. Arlequin suite ver 3.5: A new series of programs to perform population genetics analyses under Linux and Windows. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 10, 564–567 (2010).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    88.Hasegawa, M., Kishino, H. & Yano, T. A. Dating of the huma-ape splitting by a molecular clock of mitochondrial DNA. J. Mol. Evol. 22, 160–174 (1985).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    89.Kimura, M. A simple method for estimating evolutionary rate of base substitutions through comparative studies of nucletide sequences. J. Mol. Evol. 16, 111–120 (1980).ADS 
    CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    90.Felsenstein, J. Confidence limits on phylogenies: An approach using the bootstrap. Evolution 39, 783–791 (1985).PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    91.Bandelt, H. J., Forster, P. & Röhl, A. Median-joining networks for inferring intraspecific phylogenies. Mol. Biol. Evol. 16, 37–48 (1999).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    92.Dupanloup, I., Schneider, S. & Excoffier, L. A simulated annealing approach to define the genetic structure of populations. Mol. Ecol. 11, 2571–2581 (2002).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    93.Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B Stat. Methodol. 57, 289–300 (1995).MathSciNet 
    MATH 

    Google Scholar 
    94.Nei, M. Analysis of gene diversity in subdivided populations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 70, 3321–3323 (1973).ADS 
    CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    MATH 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    95.Lynch, M. & Crease, T. The analysis of population survey data on DNA sequence variation. Mol. Biol. Evol. 7, 377–394 (1990).CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 
    96.Hudson, R. R., Slatkin, M. & Maddison, W. P. Estimation of levels of gene flow from DNA sequence data. Genetics 132, 583–589 (1992).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    97.Tajima, F. Statistical method for testing the neutral mutation hypothesis by DNA polymorphism. Genetics 123, 585–595 (1989).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    98.Fu, Y.-X. Statistical tests of neutrality of mutations against population growth, hitchhiking and background selection. Genetics 147, 915–925 (1997).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    99.Harpending, H. Infertility and forager demography. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 93, 385–390 (1994).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    100.Slatkin, M. & Hudson, R. R. Pairwise comparisons of mitochondrial DNA sequences in stable and exponentially growing populations. Genetics 129, 555–562 (1991).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    101.Rogers, A. R. & Harpending, H. Population growth makes waves in the distribution of pairwise genetic differences. Mol. Biol. Evol. 9, 552–569 (1992).CAS 
    PubMed 

    Google Scholar 
    102.Schneider, S. & Excoffier, L. Estimation of past demographic parameters from the distribution of pairwise differences when the mutation rates vary among sites: Application to human mitochondrial DNA. Genetics 152, 1079–1089 (1999).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    103.Yildirim, Y. Genetic structure of Pleurobranchaea maculata in New Zealand (Massey University, 2016).
    Google Scholar 
    104.Hubbs, C. & Lagler, K. The fishes of the Great Lakes region 213 (The University of Michigan Press, 1958).
    Google Scholar 
    105.Kishimoto, H. A new stargazer, Uranoscopus flavipinnis, from Japan and Taiwan with redescription and neotype designation of U. japonicus. Japan. J. Ichthyol. 34, 1–14 (1987).
    Google Scholar 
    106.Kishimoto, H. Redescription and lectotype designation of the stargazer Uranoscopus kaianus Günther. Copeia 1984, 1009–1011 (1984).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    107.Gomon, M. F. & Johnson, J. A new fringed stargazer (Uranoscopidae: Ichthyscopus) with descriptions of the other Australian species of the genus. Mem. Queensl. Mus. 43, 597–619 (1999).
    Google Scholar 
    108.Rainboth, W. J. Fishes of the Cambodian Mekong (Food and Agriculture Org, 1996).
    Google Scholar 
    109.Imamura, H. & Matsuura, K. Redefinition and phylogenetic relationships of the family Pinguipedidae (Teleostei: Perciformes). Ichthyol. Res. 50, 259–269 (2003).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    110.Hammer, Ø., Harper, D. A. & Ryan, P. D. PAST: Paleontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontol. Electron. 4, 9 (2001).
    Google Scholar 
    111.Seah, Y. G., Nabilsyafiq, M. & Mazlan, A. G. Preliminary study on the morphology and biology of coexist Nemipterus furcosus and Nemipterus tambuloides from Terengganu Waters Peninsular Malaysia. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 11, 418–424 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    112.Johnson, R. A. & Wichern, D. W. Multivariate statistical analysis (Prentice Hall Upper Saddle River, 1998).MATH 

    Google Scholar  More

  • in

    Functional differences between TSHR alleles associate with variation in spawning season in Atlantic herring

    AnimalsTissue samples for expression analysis were collected on August 24 and September 9 2016 from a population of spring-spawning herring kept in captivity at University of Bergen; the rearing of captive herring was approved by the Norwegian national animal ethics committee (Forsøksdyrutvalget FOTS ID-5072). The tissue samples used for ATAC-sequencing were collected at Hästskär on June 19 2019 from wild spring-spawning Atlantic herring in the Baltic Sea, which do not require ethical permission.Genome scan and genetic diversity at the TSHR locusWe used a 2 × 2 contingency X2 test to estimate the extent of SNP allele frequency differences between seven spring- and seven autumn-spawning herring populations from the Northeast Atlantic (Supplementary Table 2), and thus, identify the major genomic regions associated with seasonal reproduction. The SNP allele frequencies were generated in a previous study16 and were derived from Pool-seq data. For the X2 test, we formed two groups, spring and autumn spawners, and summed the reads supporting the reference and the alternative alleles for the pools included in each group.To characterize genetic diversity at the TSHR locus, we calculated nucleotide diversity (π) and Tajima’s D for the same seven spring- and seven autumn-spawning Atlantic herring populations used in the genome scan (n = ~41–100 per pool) (Supplementary Table 2). The whole-genome re-sequence data of these pools were previously reported by Han et al.16 (for details of the pools used here see Supplementary Table 2). Unbiased nucleotide diversity π and Tajima’s D were calculated for each pool using the program PoPoolation 1.2.236, which accounts for the truncated allele frequency spectrum of pooled data. In brief, a pileup file of chromosome 15 was generated from each pool BAM file using samtools v.1.1037,38. Indels and 5 bp around indels were removed to exclude spurious SNPs due to misalignments around indels. To minimize biases in the π and Tajima’s D calculations, which are sensitive to sequencing errors and coverage fluctuations39, the coverage of each pileup file was subsampled without replacement to a uniform value based on a per-pool coverage distribution (the target coverage corresponded to the 5th percentile of the coverage frequency distribution, which was used as the minimum coverage allowed for an SNP to be included in the analysis) (Supplementary Table 2). We also calculated the diversity parameters but skipping the coverage subsampling step and obtained very similar results with both approaches (Supplementary Fig. 5), thus, we decided to keep working with the subsampled datasets as coverage subsampling is recommended by the software developers36. To exclude spurious SNPs associated with repetitive sequences and copy number variants, we applied a maximum coverage filter equivalent to the per-pool 99th percentile of the coverage frequency distribution (Supplementary Table 2). A minimum base quality of 20, a minimum mapping quality of 20, and a minor allele count of 2 were required to retain high quality SNPs for further analysis. Both, π and Tajima’s D statistics were calculated using a sliding window approach with a window size of 10 kb and a step size of 2 kb (the selected window-step combination offered a good genomic resolution while reducing the noise from single SNPs, after testing windows of 5, 10, 20, 40, 50, and 100 kb for non-overlapping and overlapping windows with a step size equivalent to 20% of the window size). Only windows with a coverage fraction ≥ 0.5 were included in the computations. In addition, we estimated the effective allele frequency difference, or delta allele frequency (dAF), between spring and autumn spawning groups at the TSHR locus using the formula dAF = abs(mean(spring pools)−mean(autumn pools)). For each of the diversity parameters, we assessed whether the mean differences between sets of SNPs within chr 15: 8.85–8.95 Mb (215 SNPs) and outside (214 635 SNPs) the TSHR region were statistically significant among spring- and autumn-spawning groups using a Wilcoxon test. Data postprocessing, statistical tests, and plotting were performed in the R environment40 (for specific parameters used in PoPoolation, see the associated code to this publication).Identification of the 5.2 kb structural variantSequences spanning the entire TSHR gene plus 10 kb upstream and downstream from two reference assemblies, ASM96633v115 and Ch_v2.0.218, were aligned using BLAST41 and the output was subsequently processed with a custom R script40. Repeats were annotated by CENSOR21 for the region harboring the 5.2 kb structural variant. To validate the structural variant, long-range PCR was performed with genomic DNA from two spring- and autumn-spawning Atlantic herring in a 20 μL reaction containing 0.8 mM dNTPs, 0.3 μM each of the forward and reverse 5.2kb-confirm primers (Supplementary Table 1) and 0.75 U PrimeSTAR GXL DNA Polymerase (TaKaRa) following the program: 95 °C for 3 min, 35 cycles of 98 °C for 10 s, 58 °C for 20 s and 68 °C for 2 min 30 s, and a final extension of 10 min at 68 °C.ATAC-seq analysisBSH and brain without BSH were dissected from two spring-spawning herring caught in the Baltic Sea and transported to the lab on dry ice, then kept at –80 °C before nuclei isolation. ATAC-seq libraries were constructed according to the Omni-ATAC protocol with minor modifications42. Briefly, tissue was homogenized in 2 ml homogenization buffer (5 mM CaCl2, 3 mM Mg(Ac)2, 10 mM Tris-HCl (pH = 7.8), 0.017 mM PMSF, 0.17 mM ß-mercaptoethanol, 320 mM Sucrose, 0.1 mM EDTA and 0.1% NP-40) with a Dounce homogenizer on ice. 400 μl suspension was transferred to a 2 ml tube for the density gradient centrifugation with different concentrations of Iodixanol solution. After centrifugation, a 200 μl fraction containing the nuclei band was collected, stained with Trypan blue and counted with a Countess II Automated Cell Counter (Thermo Fisher Scientific). An aliquot of 100,000–200,000 nuclei was used as input in a 50 μl transposition reaction containing 2 X TD buffer and 100 nM assembled Tn5 transposase for a 30-min incubation at 37 °C. Tagmented DNA was purified with a Zymo clean kit (Zymo Research). Purified DNA was used for an initial pre-amplification for 5 cycles, and the additional amplification cycle was determined by qPCR based on the “R vs Cycle Number” plot43. Amplified libraries were purified with a Zymo clean kit again, and library concentrations and qualities were evaluated using the 2200 TapeStation System (Agilent Technologies).ATAC-seq was performed with a MiniSeq High Output Kit (150 cycles) on a MiniSeq instrument (Illumina) and 7–9 million reads were generated for each ATAC-seq library. Quality control, trimming, mapping, and peak calling of the sequenced reads were conducted following the ENCODE ATAC-seq pipeline (https://www.encodeproject.org/atac-seq/). The trimmed reads were aligned to the Atlantic herring reference genome (Ch_v2.0.2)18 with Bowtie244 and the mapping rate was 85–95%. Duplicate reads, reads with low mapping quality and those aligned to the mitochondria genome were removed. The remaining reads (4–5 million) were subjected to peak calling by MACS245, where 22–32 K peaks were called. Sequenced library qualities were further evaluated by calculating the TSS enrichment score and checking the library complexity with the Non-Redundant Fraction (NRF) and PCR Bottlenecking Coefficients (PBC1 and 2). Finally, conserved peaks between two biological replicates were identified by evaluating the irreproducible discovery rate (IDR).Genotyping of six differentiated variants and haplotype analysisAll six genetic variants, including the 5.2 kb structural variant, two non-coding SNPs, two missense SNPs and the copy number variant of C-terminal 22aa repeat, were genotyped in 45 spring-, 67 autumn-spawning Atlantic herring and 13 Pacific herring. TaqMan Custom SNP assays were performed to genotype the four SNPs in 5 μl reactions with a template of 20 ng genomic DNA (ThermoFisher Scientific). Copy number of the C-terminal 22aa repeat was determined by the PCR product size generated with geno22aa primers. Genotyping of the 5.2 kb structural variant was performed in a PCR reaction containing two forward primers (geno5.2kb-1F and geno5.2kb-2F) and one reverse primer (geno5.2kb-R), which generated PCR products with different sizes between spring and autumn spawners. All the primers used for genotyping are listed in Supplementary Table 1.Tissue expression profiles by quantitative PCRTotal RNA was prepared from gonad, heart, spleen, kidney, gills, intestine, hypothalamus and saccus vasculosus (BSH), and brain without BSH (brain) of six adult spring-spawning Atlantic herring using RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen). RNA was then reverse transcribed into cDNA with a High-Capacity cDNA Reverse Transcription Kit (ThermoFisher Scientific). TaqMan Gene Expression assay (ThermoFisher Scientific) containing 0.3 μM primers and 0.25 μM TaqMan probe (Integrated DNA Technologies) was performed to compare the relative expression levels of TSHR among different tissues. qPCR with SYBR Green chemistry was used for TSHB and DIO2 in a 10 μl reaction of SYBR Green PCR Master Mix (ThermoFisher Scientific) and 0.3 μM primers, with a program composed of an initial denaturation for 10 min at 95 °C followed by 40 cycles of 95 °C for 15 s and 60 °C for 1 min. Ct values were first normalized to the housekeeping gene ACTIN, then the average expression for each gene in the gonad was assumed to be 1 for the subsequent calculation of the relative expression in other tissues.Plasmid constructsThe coding sequence for the herring single-chain TSH (scTSH) was designed following a strategy previously used for mammalian gonadotropins46 that contained an in-frame fusion of the cDNA sequences (5′–3′) of herring TSH beta subunit (NCBI: XM_012836756.1) and alpha subunit (NCBI: XM_012822755.1) linked by six histidines and then the C-terminal peptide of the hCG beta subunit. The designed sequence should generate a protein with a size of 30.6 kDa. Both scTSH and spring herring TSHR cDNA sequences were synthesized in vitro and cloned in the expression vector pcDNA3.1 by Genscript (Leiden, Netherlands). pcDNA3.1 plasmid expressing human TSHR was kindly provided by Drs. Gilbert Vassart and Sabine Costagliola (Université libre de Bruxelles, Belgium). Then, the spring herring TSHR and human TSHR plasmids were used as templates for site-directed mutagenesis to generate constructs coding for different mutant herring or human TSHRs. Plasmids for the dual-luciferase assay, including pGL4.29[luc2P/CRE/Hygro] containing cAMP response elements (CREs) to drive the transcription of luciferase gene luc2P and pRL-TK monitoring the transfection efficiency, were purchased from Promega. Five ng of each plasmid was used to transform the XL1-Blue competent cells (Agilent), plasmid DNA was subsequently extracted from 200 ml overnight culture of a single transformant clone using an EndoFree plasmid Maxi Kit (Qiagen).Cell cultureChinese hamster ovary (CHO) (ATCC CCL-61) and human embryonic kidney 293 (HEK293) (ATCC CRL-1573) cells were maintained in DMEM supplemented with 5% (CHO) or 10% FBS (HEK293), 100 U/ml penicillin, 100 μg/ml streptomycin and 292 μg/ml l-Glutamine (ThermoFisher Scientific) at 37 °C with 5% CO2. Epithelioma Papulosum Cyprini (EPC) cells (ATCC CRL-2872) were cultured in EMEM (Sigma) supplemented with 10% FBS, 100 U/ml penicillin, 100 μg/ml streptomycin, 292 μg/ml l-Glutamine and 1 mM Sodium Pyruvate (ThermoFisher Scientific) at 26 °C with 5% CO2.Production of recombinant herring scTSHCHO cells were transfected with the scTSH expression plasmid using Lipofectamine 3000 (Invitrogen), stable clones were subsequently selected with 500 μg/ml G418 (Invitrogen) and screened for producing scTSH by western blot using a polyclonal antisera against the sea bass alpha subunit47. A positive clone was expanded in 225 cm2 cell culture flasks (Corning) in culture medium containing 5% FBS until confluence, then the cells were maintained in serum-free DMEM for hormone production for 7 days at 25 °C48. After 7 days, culture medium containing scTSH or without (negative control) was centrifuged at 15000 x g for 15 min and concentrated by ultrafiltration using Centricon Plus-70 / Ultracel PL-30 (Merck Millipore Ltd.). Then, western blotting was performed to confirm TSH production. Concentrated medium containing herring scTSH was denatured at 94 °C for 5 min in 0.1% SDS and 50 mM 2-mercaptoethanol, and then treated with 2.5 units of peptide-N-glycosidase F (Roche Diagnostics) at 37 °C for 2 h in 20 mM sodium phosphate with 0.5% Nonidet P-40, pH 7.5. All samples were run in 12% SDS-PAGE in the reducing condition and transferred to a PVDF membrane (Immobilon P; Millipore Corp.), then blocked overnight with 5% skimmed milk at 4 °C. After blocking, the membrane was incubated with polyclonal antisera against the sea bass alpha subunit (dilution 1:2000) for 90 min at room temperature, washed, and then further incubated with 1:25000 goat anti-rabbit immunoglobulin G (IgG) horseradish peroxidase conjugate (Bio-Rad Laboratories) for 60 min at room temperature. Immunodetection was performed by chemiluminescence with a Pierce ECL Plus Western Blotting Substrate kit (ThermoFisher Scientific).Cell surface expressionA Rhotag (MNGTEGPNFYVPFSNKTGVVYEE) was inserted at the N-terminus of herring TSHR for flow cytometry analysis of receptor cell surface expression. Anti-Rhotag polyclonal antibody was kindly provided by Drs. Gilbert Vassart and Sabine Costagliola (Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium). PBS containing 1% BSA and 0.05% sodium azide was prepared as the flow cytometry (FCM) buffer for the washing and antibody incubation steps. 2.2 × 106 EPC cells were seeded in a 100 mm poly-d-Lysine-treated petri dish the day before transfection. Each dish was transfected with 10 μg TSHR or empty pcDNA3.1 expression plasmid using 20 μl jetPRIME transfection reagent in 500 μl jetPRIME transfection buffer (Polyplus transfection). Cells were harvested 24 h after transfection, then washed once in cold PBS and fixed in 2% PFA for 10 min at room temperature. After fixation, cells were washed three times with FCM buffer, then incubated with anti-Rhotag antibody or FCM buffer (negative control) for 1 h at room temperature. Cells were washed again with FCM buffer three times and stained with Alexa Fluor 488-labeled chicken anti-mouse IgG (H + L) antibody (1:200 dilution, ThermoFisher Scientific) or FCM buffer (negative control) for 45 min in the dark. After the fluorescent staining, cells were washed three times and resuspended in FCM buffer before analysis on a CytoFLEX instrument (Beckman Coulter). A minimum of 100,000 events was recorded for each sample, fluorescence intensities of negative control and cells transfected with empty pcDNA3.1 plasmid were used as the background for gating strategy. Cell surface expression was represented by the mean fluorescence intensity of the positively stained cell population.Dual-luciferase reporter assayEPC or HEK293 cells were plated in a 48-well plate at a density of 1 × 105 cells/well the day before transfection. A total of 250 ng plasmid mixture containing pGL4.29[luc2P/CRE/Hygro], TSHR expression plasmid (or empty pcDNA3.1) and pRL-TK with the ratio of 20:5:1 was prepared to transfect each well of cells using jetPRIME transfection reagent (Polyplus). Medium was replaced by fresh medium containing 10% FBS (TSH-induced condition) or serum-free medium (constitutive activity condition) 4 h after transfection. On day three, cells were treated with serum-free medium containing different dilutions of the concentrated scTSH medium for 4 h (TSH-induced condition) or directly subjected to the luminescence measurement without TSH induction (constitutive activity condition). Luminescence was measured using a Dual-Luciferase Reporter assay (Promega) on an Infinite M200 Microplate Reader (Tecan Group Ltd., Switzerland), and luciferase activity was represented as the ratio of firefly (pGL4.29[luc2P/CRE/Hygro]) to Renilla (pRL-TK) luminescence.5′-RACE to identify the herring DIO2 TSSTotal RNA was prepared from brain of a spring-spawning Atlantic herring using the RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen). Six μg of the isolated RNA was used for 5′-RACE with a FirstChoiceTM RLM-RACE Kit (ThermoFisher Scientific). One μl cDNA or Outer RACE PCR product was used as PCR template in a 20 μL reaction containing 0.8 mM dNTPs, 0.3 μM of each forward and reverse primer (Supplementary Table 1) and 0.75 U PrimeSTAR GXL DNA Polymerase (TaKaRa). Amplification was carried out with an initial denaturation of 3 min at 95 °C, followed by 35 cycles of 98 °C for 10 s, 58 °C for 20 s and 68 °C for 40 s, and a final extension of 10 min at 68 °C. The final 5′ RACE product was sequenced at Eurofins Genomics (Ebersberg, Germany).Sequence conservation analysisGenomic sequences covering the TSHR locus were extracted from Ensembl Genome Browser for Atlantic herring and 11 other fish species, including Amazon molly (Poecilia formosa), denticle herring (Denticeps clupeoides), goldfish (Carassius auratus), guppy (Poecilia reticulata), Neolamprologus brichardi, Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes), northern pike (Esox lucius), orange clownfish (Amphiprion percula), spotted gar (Lepisosteus oculatus), three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) and spotted green pufferfish (Tetraodon nigroviridis). The extracted sequences were firstly aligned using progressiveCactus49,50, and a subsequent alignment was generated using the hal2maf program from halTools51 with Atlantic herring assembly (Ch_v2.0.2)18 as the co-ordinate backbone. This alignment was used for the downstream phastCons score calculation by running phyloFit24 and phastCons25 from the PHAST package with default parameters. Peaks were called by grouping signals with a minimum phastCons score of 0.2 within 500 bp region.Structure modeling of human and herring TSHRsIn order to explore the possible interactions of the variant residues with other receptor interacting proteins and to study intramolecular interactions, we built a structural homology model for the herring TSHR (herrTSHR) complexed with herring TSH and Gs-protein. The TSHR hinge region that harbors the Q370H substitution and the C-terminus containing the 22aa repeat were excluded from the homology model due to the lack of structural templates for these regions. The homology model was constructed by using the following structural templates of evolutionarily related class A GPCRs: (i) the leucine-rich repeat domain (LRRD) complexed with hormone was modeled based on the solved FSHR LRRD – FSH complex structure (Protein Data Bank (PDB) ID: 4AY9)52,53, this part of model included herring TSHR Cys33 – Asn296 and fragments of the hinge region Gln297 – Thr312 and Ser393 – Ile421; (ii) the available structural complex of β2-adrenoreceptor with Gs-protein (PDB ID: 3SN6)54 was used as the template to model the seven-transmembrane helix domain (7TMD) of herring TSHR in the active conformation; (iii) the extracellular loop 2 (ECL2) was built by using the ECL2 of μ-opioid receptor (PDB ID: 6DDE)55. To prepare the template for herring TSHR modeling, the fused T4-lysozyme and bound ligand of β2-adrenoreceptor were deleted, the ECL1 and ECL3 loops were adjusted manually to the loop length of herring TSHR. Due to the lack of third intracellular loop (ICL3) in the β2-adrenoreceptor structure, amino acid residues of herring TSHR ICL3 were manually added to the template. Since herring TSHR does not have the TMH5 proline, which is highly conserved among all class A GPCRs and responsible for the helical kinks and bulges within this region56, we assumed a rather regular (stretched) helix conformation for the herring TSHR TMH5 and therefore replaced the kinked β2-adrenoreceptor TMH5 template with a regular α-helix. Moreover, the ECL2 template was substituted with μOR ECL2 structure because of its higher sequence similarity with herring TSHR in this region. Finally, amino acid residues of this chimeric 7TMD template and FSHR N-terminus were mutated to the corresponding spring herring TSHR residues and sequence of the heterodimeric FSH ligand was substituted by the herring TSH. All homology models were generated by using SYBYL-X 2.0 (Certara, NJ, US). The 7TMD structure was then fused with FSHR N-terminus at position 421. The assembled complex was subsequently optimized by the energy minimization under constrained backbone atoms (the AMBER F99 force field was used), followed by a 2 ns molecular dynamics simulation (MD) of the side chains. The entire TSHR complex was energetically minimized without any constraints until converging at a termination gradient of 0.05 kcal/mol*Å. Next, for autumn herrTSHR modeling, the spring TSHR sequence was substituted with autumn TSHR sequence. For humTSHR, the spring TSHR sequence was substituted with human TSHR, and the herring TSH ligand was replaced by the bovine TSH sequence. Both complex models were energetically minimized until converging at a termination gradient of 0.05 kcal/mol*Å.To investigate the microenvironment around the L471M mutation at TMH2 position 2.51, local short MD’s of 4 ns on Met4712.51 (spring herrTSHR), Leu471 (autumn herrTSHR) or Phe461 (humTSHR) and its surrounding amino acids were performed. During MD simulations, backbone atoms of the entire complexes as well as all side chains, except residues at positions 1.47, 1.51, 1.54, 2.48, 2.52, and 2.55 that form the hydrophobic patch around position 2.51, were constrained.Statistics and reproducibilityResults were presented as the mean + SD (standard deviation) calculated from at least four biological replicates for each experiment, and at least two independent experiments were conducted for each assay. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t test was performed to calculate the P-values and means were judged as statistically significant when P ≤ 0.05.Reporting summaryFurther information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article. More

  • in

    Multi-year presence of humpback whales in the Atlantic sector of the Southern Ocean but not during El Niño

    1.Clapham, P. J. in Encyclopedia of marine mammals 489–492 (Elsevier, 2018).2.Stevick, P. T. et al. A quarter of a world away: female humpback whale moves 10 000 km between breeding areas. Biol. Lett. 7, 299–302 (2010).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    3.Nicol, S. et al. Southern Ocean iron fertilization by baleen whales and Antarctic krill. Fish. Fish. 11, 203–209 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    4.Smetacek, V. & Nicol, S. Polar ocean ecosystems in a changing world. Nature 437, 362–368 (2005).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    5.Dunlop, R. A. Potential motivational information encoded within humpback whale non-song vocal sounds. J. Acoustical Soc. Am. 141, 2204–2213 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    6.Stimpert, A. K., Au, W. W. L., Parks, S. E., Hurst, T. & Wiley, D. N. Common humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) sound types for passive acoustic monitoring. J. Acoustical Soc. Am. 129, 476–482 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    7.Van Opzeeland, I., Van Parijs, S., Kindermann, L., Burkhardt, E. & Boebel, O. Calling in the cold: pervasive acoustic presence of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in Antarctic coastal waters. PLoS ONE 8, 1–7 (2013).
    Google Scholar 
    8.Siegel, V. Biology and Ecology of Antarctic Krill. (Springer, 2016).9.Atkinson, A. et al. Krill (Euphausia superba) distribution contracts southward during rapid regional warming. Nat. Clim. Change 9, 142–147 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    10.Loeb, V. J., Hofmann, E. E., Klinck, J. M., Holm-Hansen, O. & White, W. B. ENSO and variability of the Antarctic Peninsula pelagic marine ecosystem. Antarctic Sci. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954102008001636 (2009).11.Loeb, V. J. & Santora, J. A. Climate variability and spatiotemporal dynamics of five Southern Ocean krill species. Prog. Oceanogr. 134, 93–122 (2015).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    12.Bombosch, A. et al. Predictive habitat modelling of humpback (Megaptera novaeangliae) and Antarctic minke (Balaenoptera bonaerensis) whales in the Southern Ocean as a planning tool for seismic surveys. Deep-Sea Res. Part I: Oceanographic Res. Pap. 91, 101–114 (2014).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    13.Brierley, A. S. et al. Antarctic krill under sea ice: elevated abundance in a narrow band just south of ice edge. Science 295, 1890–1892 (2002).CAS 
    PubMed 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    14.Rettig, S. et al. in 1st International Conference and Exhibition on Underwater Acoustics. (eds Papadakis, J. & Bjorno, L.) 1669–1674 (2013).15.Garland, E. C. et al. Humpback whale song on the Southern Ocean feeding grounds: Implications for cultural transmission. PLoS ONE 8, e79422 (2013).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    16.Stimpert, A. K., Peavey, L. E., Friedlaender, A. S. & Nowacek, D. P. Humpback whale song and foraging behavior on an Antarctic feeding ground. PLoS ONE 7, e51214 (2012).CAS 
    PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    17.Filun, D. et al. Frozen verses: Antarctic minke whales (Balaenoptera bonaerensis) call predominantly during austral winter. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 192112 (2020).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    18.Boebel, O. The Expedition PS89 of the Research Vessel POLARSTERN to the Weddell Sea in 2014/2015. (Alfred Wegener Institute, Helmholtz Centre for Polar and Marine Research, Bremerhaven, 2015).19.Burkhardt, E. Whale sightings during Polarstern cruise PS96 (ANT-XXXI/2), https://doi.org/10.1594/PANGAEA.923113 (2020).20.Herr, H., Viquerat, S. & Siebert, U. Aerial cetacean survey Southern Ocean 2014/2015, https://doi.org/10.1594/PANGAEA.894938 (2018).21.Herr, H., Viquerat, S. & Siebert, U. Ship based cetacean survey Southern Ocean 2014/2015, https://doi.org/10.1594/PANGAEA.894873 (2018).22.National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration & Department of Commerce. Climate Prediction Centre (CPC) Oceanic Nino Index (2019).23.Thomisch, K. et al. Spatio-temporal patterns in acoustic presence and distribution of Antarctic blue whales Balaenoptera musculus intermedia in the Weddell Sea. Endanger. Species Res. 30, 239–253 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    24.Širović, A. et al. Seasonality of blue and fin whale calls and the influence of sea ice in the Western Antarctic Peninsula. Deep Sea Res. Part II: Topical Stud. Oceanogr. 51, 2327–2344 (2004).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    25.Schall, E. et al. Large-scale spatial variabilities in the humpback whale acoustic presence in the Atlantic sector of the Southern Ocean. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 201347 (2020).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    26.Loeb, V., Hofmann, E. E., Klinck, J. M. & Holm-Hansen, O. Hydrographic control of the marine ecosystem in the South Shetland-Elephant Island and Bransfield Strait region. Deep Sea Res. Part II: Topical Stud. Oceanogr. 57, 519–542 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    27.Sallée, J.-B., Speer, K. & Rintoul, S. Zonally asymmetric response of the Southern Ocean mixed-layer depth to the Southern Annular Mode. Nat. Geosci. 3, 273–279 (2010).Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    28.Kim, Y. S. & Orsi, A. H. On the variability of Antarctic Circumpolar Current fronts inferred from 1992–2011 altimetry. J. Phys. Oceanogr. 44, 3054–3071 (2014).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    29.Yuan, X. ENSO-related impacts on Antarctic sea ice: a synthesis of phenomenon and mechanisms. Antarct. Sci. 16, 415 (2004).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    30.Meredith, M. P., Murphy, E. J., Hawker, E. J., King, J. C. & Wallace, M. I. On the interannual variability of ocean temperatures around South Georgia, Southern Ocean: Forcing by El Niño/Southern Oscillation and the southern annular mode. Deep Sea Res. Part II: Topical Stud. Oceanogr. 55, 2007–2022 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    31.Lovenduski, N. S. & Gruber, N. Impact of the Southern Annular Mode on Southern Ocean circulation and biology. Geophys. Res. Lett. 32, 1–4 (2005).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    32.Craig, A. S., Herman, L. M., Gabriele, C. M. & Pack, A. A. Migratory timing of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in the central north Pacific varies with age, sex and reproductive status. Behaviour 140, 981–1001 (2003).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    33.Hofmann, E. E., Klinck, J. M., Locarnini, R. A., Fach, B. & Murphy, E. Krill transport in the Scotia Sea and environs. Antarct. Sci. 10, 406–415 (1998).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    34.Barendse, J. et al. Migration redefined? Seasonality, movements and group composition of humpback whales Megaptera novaeangliae off the west coast of South Africa. Afr. J. Mar. Sci. 32, 1–22 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    35.Witteveen, B. H., Foy, R. J., Wynne, K. M. & Tremblay, Y. Investigation of foraging habits and prey selection by humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) using acoustic tags and concurrent fish surveys. Mar. Mammal. Sci. 24, 516–534 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    36.Brown, M. R., Corkeron, P. J., Hale, P. T., Schultz, K. W. & Bryden, M. M. Evidence for a sex-segregated migration in the humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 259, 229–234 (1995).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    37.International Whaling Commission. Report on the workshop on the comprehensive assessment of Southern Hemisphere humpback whales. J. Cetacea. Res. Manag. Spec. Issue 3, 1–50 (2011).
    Google Scholar 
    38.Findlay, K. P. et al. Humpback whale “super-groups” – A novel low-latitude feeding behaviour of Southern Hemisphere humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in the Benguela Upwelling System. PLOS ONE 12, e0172002 (2017).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 
    CAS 

    Google Scholar 
    39.Gridley, T., Silva, M., Wilkinson, C., Seakamela, S. & Elwen, S. H. Song recorded near a super-group of humpback whales on a mid-latitude feeding ground off South Africa. J. Acoustical Soc. Am. 143, EL298–EL304 (2018).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    40.Ross-Marsh, E., Elwen, S., Prinsloo, A., James, B. & Gridley, T. Singing in South Africa: monitoring the occurrence of humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) song near the Western Cape. Bioacoustics, 1–17, https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2019.1710254 (2020).41.Cai, W. et al. Increasing frequency of extreme El Niño events due to greenhouse warming. Nat. Clim. Change 4, 111–116 (2014).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    42.Bengtson Nash, S. M. et al. Signals from the south; humpback whales carry messages of Antarctic sea‐ice ecosystem variability. Glob. Change Biol. 24, 1500–1510 (2018).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    43.Baumgartner, M. F. & Mussoline, S. E. A generalized baleen whale call detection and classification system. J. Acoustical Soc. Am. 129, 2889–2902 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    44.Klinck, H. et al. Long-range underwater vocalizations of the crabeater seal (Lobodon carcinophaga). J. Acoustical Soc. Am. 128, 474–479 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    45.Risch, D. et al. Mysterious bio-duck sound attributed to the Antarctic minke whale (Balaenoptera bonaerensis). Biol. Lett. 10, 20140175 (2014).PubMed 
    PubMed Central 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    46.Schall, E. & Van Opzeeland, I. Calls produced by Ecotype C killer whales (Orcinus orca) off the Eckstrom iceshelf, Antarctica. Aquat. Mamm. 43, 117–126 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    47.Van Opzeeland, I. et al. Acoustic ecology of Antarctic pinnipeds. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 414, 267–291 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    48.Dunlop, R. A., Cato, D. H. & Noad, M. J. Non-song acoustic communication in migrating humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). Mar. Mammal. Sci. 24, 613–629 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    49.Schall, E. & El-Gabbas, A. Humpback-whale-acoustic-detection-and-environmental-modelling, https://github.com/elenaschall/Humpback-whale-acoustic-detection-and-environmental-modelling (GitHub, GitHub, 2021).50.Bioacoustics, Research & Program. Raven Pro: Interactive Sound Analysis Software (Version 1.5) http://ravensoundsoftware.com/ (The Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, 2014).51.Cavalieri, D., Parkinson, C., Gloersen, P. & Zwally, H. Sea ice concentrations from Nimbus-7 SMMR and DMSP SSM/I-SSMIS passive microwave data, version 1. Boulder, Colorado USA, NASA National Snow and Ice Data Center Distributed Active Archive Center 10, https://doi.org/10.5067/8GQ8LZQVL0VL (1996).52.Greene, C. A. Daily Antarctic sea ice concentration (2020).53.Marshall, G. J. Trends in the southern annular mode from observations and reanalyses. J. Clim. 16, 4134–4143 (2003).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    54.Marshall, G. & National Center for Atmospheric Research Staff (Eds). The climate data guide: Marshall Southern Annular Mode (SAM) index (Station-based), (2019).55.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing, https://www.R-project.org/ (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 2018)56.National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) & Climate Prediction Centre (CPC). Oceanic Nino Index (2019).57.Wood, S. N. Generalized additive models: an introduction with R (CRC press, 2017).58.Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S. & Sarkar, D. _nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-145 (R CoreTeam, 2020).59.Hyndman, R. et al. forecast: Forecasting functions for time series and linear models_. R. package version 8.11, http://pkg.robjhyndman.com/forecast (2020)..60.Schall, E. et al. Humpback whale acoustic presence in the Atlantic sector of the Southern Ocean, https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.ncjsxkss0 (2021).61.Wessel, P. & Smith, W. H. A global, self‐consistent, hierarchical, high‐resolution shoreline database. J. Geophys. Res.: Solid Earth 101, 8741–8743 (1996).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    62.Amante, C. & Eakins, B. W. ETOPO1 arc-minute global relief model: procedures, data sources and analysis, https://doi.org/10.7289/V5C8276M (2009).63.Spreen, G., Kaleschke, L. & Heygster, G. Sea ice remote sensing using AMSR-E 89-GHz channels. J. Geophys Res. Oceans 113, C02S03 (2008).Article 

    Google Scholar  More

  • in

    Red light, green light: both signal ‘go’ to deadly algae

    Green and red lighting might be good for migratory birds and sea turtles, but could have undesirable effects if marine algae are present. Credit: Getty

    Ecology
    24 June 2021
    Red light, green light: both signal ‘go’ to deadly algae

    Artificial lighting thought to be more wildlife-friendly than white light could encourage algal blooms.

    Share on Twitter
    Share on Twitter

    Share on Facebook
    Share on Facebook

    Share via E-Mail
    Share via E-Mail

    Green or red lights in seaside areas have been proposed as alternatives to white light to protect wildlife. But new experiments show that exposure to red or green light at night boosts the growth of some ocean algae — including species known to rob waters of oxygen.Little is known about the impact of artificial light on marine life, even though many brightly lit cities are coastal. To address that knowledge gap, Sofie Spatharis at the University of Glasgow, UK, and her colleagues exposed a mix of microscopic marine algae collected from Scottish waters to standard white light. They also exposed the mixture to red and green lights, which have been proposed to minimize impacts on sea turtles and migratory seabirds, respectively.The team found that all light colours enhanced growth of the microalgae mix. Red light had the most pronounced effect, doubling the number of cells produced. The proportions of species in the mixture also shifted: both red and green light especially favoured growth of harmful species in the Skeletonema genus, which form dense blooms that are deadly to fish.

    Proc. R. Soc. B (2021)

    Ecology More

  • in

    Random population fluctuations bias the Living Planet Index

    1.Mace, G. M. et al. Aiming higher to bend the curve of biodiversity loss. Nat. Sustain. 1, 448–451 (2018).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    2.Leclère, D. et al. Bending the curve of terrestrial biodiversity needs an integrated strategy. Nature 585, 551–556 (2020).PubMed 

    Google Scholar 
    3.Updated Zero Draft of the Post-2020 Global Biodiversity Framework (Convention on Biological Diversity, 2020); https://www.cbd.int/doc/c/3064/749a/0f65ac7f9def86707f4eaefa/post2020-prep-02-01-en.pdf4.Pereira, H. M. et al. Essential biodiversity variables. Science 339, 277–278 (2013).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    5.Loh, J. et al. The Living Planet Index: using species population time series to track trends in biodiversity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 360, 289–295 (2005).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    6.Collen, B. et al. Monitoring change in vertebrate abundance: the Living Planet Index. Conserv. Biol. 23, 317–327 (2009).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    7.McRae, L., Deinet, S. & Freeman, R. The diversity-weighted Living Planet Index: controlling for taxonomic bias in a global biodiversity indicator. PLoS ONE 12, e0169156 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    8.Almond, R.E.A., Grooten M. & Petersen, T. (eds) Living Planet Report 2020—Bending the Curve of Biodiversity Loss (WWF, 2020).9.Summary for Policymakers of the Global Assessment Report on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services of the Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services (IPBES, 2019).10.Global Biodiversity Outlook 5 (Convention on Biological Diversity, 2020).11.Jaspers, A. Can a single index track the state of global biodiversity? Biol. Conserv. 246, 108524 (2020).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    12.Leung, B. et al. Clustered versus catastrophic global vertebrate declines. Nature 588, 267–271 (2020).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    13.Buckland, S. T., Studeny, A. C., Magurran, A. E., Illian, J. & Newson, S. E. The geometric mean of relative abundance indices: a biodiversity measure with a difference. Ecosphere 2, 100 (2011).14.de Valpine, P. & Hastings, A. Fitting population models incorporating process noise and observation error. Ecol. Monogr. 72, 57–76.15.Daskalova, G. N., Myers-Smith, I. H. & Godlee, J. L. Rare and common vertebrates span a wide spectrum of population trends. Nat. Commun. 11, 4394 (2020).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    16.Living Planet Report 2020. Technical Supplement: Living Planet Index (WWF, 2020); https://f.hubspotusercontent20.net/hubfs/4783129/LPR/PDFs/ENGLISH%20-%20TECH%20SUPPLIMENT.pdf17.Vellend, M. Conceptual synthesis in community ecology. Quart. Rev. Biol. 85, 183–206 (2010).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    18.Vellend, M. et al. Assessing the relative importance of neutral stochasticity in ecological communities. Oikos 123, 1420–1430 (2014).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    19.Lande, R. Risks of population extinction from demographic and environmental stochasticity and random catastrophes. Am. Nat. 142, 911–927 (1993).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    20.Gravel, D., Guichard, F. & Hochberg, M. E. Species coexistence in a variable world. Ecol. Lett. 14, 828–839 (2011).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    21.Kotze, D. J., O’Hara, R. B. & Lehvävirta, S. Dealing with varying detection probability, unequal sample sizes and clumped distributions in count data. PLoS ONE 7, e40923 (2012).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    22.Kellner, K. F. & Swihart, R. K. Accounting for imperfect detection in ecology: a quantitative review. PLoS ONE 9, e111436 (2014).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    23.Di Fonzo, M., Collen, B. & Mace, G. M. A new method for identifying rapid decline dynamics in wild vertebrate populations. Ecol. Evol. 3, 2378–2391 (2013).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    24.Maxwell, S. L. et al. Being smart about SMART environmental targets. Science 347, 1075–1076 (2015).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    25.Butchart, S. H. M., Di Marco, M. & Watson, J. E. M. Formulating SMART commitments on biodiversity: lessons from the Aichi Targets. Conserv Lett. 9, 457–468 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    26.Green, E. J. et al. Relating characteristics of global biodiversity targets to reported progress. Conserv. Biol. 33, 1360–1369 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    27.Dornelas, M. et al. A balance of winners and losers in the Anthropocene. Ecol. Lett. 22, 847–854 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    28.Fournier, A. M. V., White, E. R. & Heard, S. B. Site‐selection bias and apparent population declines in long‐term studies. Conserv. Biol. 33, 1370–1379 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    29.Pauly, D. Anecdotes and the shifting baseline syndrome of fisheries. Trends Ecol. Evol. 10, 430 (1995).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    30.Papworth, S. K., Rist, J., Coad, L. & Milner-Gulland, E. J. Evidence for shifting baseline syndrome in conservation. Conserv Lett. 2, 93–100 (2009).
    Google Scholar 
    31.Barnosky, A. D. et al. Approaching a state shift in Earth’s biosphere. Nature 486, 52–58 (2012).CAS 
    Article 

    Google Scholar 
    32.Nicholson, E. et al. Scenarios and models to support global conservation targets. Trends Ecol. Evol. 34, 57–68 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    33.Bull, J. W., Strange, N., Smith, R. J. & Gordon, A. Reconciling multiple counterfactuals when evaluating biodiversity conservation impact in social-ecological systems. Conserv. Biol. 35, 510–521 (2021).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    34.van Strien, A. J. et al. Modest recovery of biodiversity in a western European country: The Living Planet Index for the Netherlands. Biol. Conserv. 200, 44–50 (2016).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    35.Wauchope, H. S., Amano, T., Sutherland, W. J. & Johnston, A. When can we trust population trends? A method for quantifying the effects of sampling interval and duration. Methods Ecol. Evol. 10, 2067–2078 (2019).Article 

    Google Scholar 
    36.Wauchope, H. S. et al. Evaluating impact using time-series data. Trends Ecol. Evol. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2020.11.001 (2020).37.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2020).38.Buschke, F. T. Biodiversity trajectories and the time needed to achieve no net loss through averted-loss biodiversity offsets. Ecol. Model 352, 54–57 (2017).Article 

    Google Scholar  More