Great tits who remember more accurately have difficulty forgetting, but variation is not driven by environmental harshness
1.Croston, R., Branch, C. L., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Dukas, R. & Pravosudov, V. V. The importance of heritability estimates for understanding the evolution of cognition: A response to comments on Croston et al. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1463–1464 (2015).Article
Google Scholar
2.Langley, E. J. G. et al. Heritability and correlations among learning and inhibitory control traits. Behav. Ecol. 1, 1–9 (2020).
Google Scholar
3.Boogert, N. J., Madden, J. R., Morand-Ferron, J. & Thornton, A. Measuring and understanding individual differences in cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 373, 2017080 (2018).
Google Scholar
4.Sonnenberg, B. R., Branch, C. L., Pitera, A. M., Bridge, E. & Pravosudov, V. V. Natural selection and spatial cognition in wild food-caching mountain chickadees. Curr. Biol. 29, 1–7 (2019).Article
CAS
Google Scholar
5.Benedict, L. M. et al. Elevation-related differences in annual survival of adult food-caching mountain chickadees are consistent with natural selection on spatial cognition. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 74, 2817 (2020).Article
Google Scholar
6.Shaw, R. C., MacKinlay, R. D., Clayton, N. S. & Burns, K. C. Memory performance influences male reproductive success in a wild bird. Curr. Biol. 29, 1498–1502 (2019).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
7.Cauchoix, M. & Chaine, A. S. How can we study the evolution of animal minds?. Front. Psychol. 7, 1–18 (2016).Article
Google Scholar
8.Janmaat, K. R. L. et al. Spatio-temporal complexity of chimpanzee food: How cognitive adaptations can counteract the ephemeral nature of ripe fruit. Am. J. Primatol. 78, 626–645 (2016).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
9.Collett, M., Chittka, L. & Collett, T. S. Spatial memory in insect navigation. Curr. Biol. 23, R789–R800 (2013).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
10.Hampton, R. R. & Shettleworth, S. J. Hippocampus and memory in a food-storing and in a nonstoring bird species. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 946–964 (1996).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
11.LaDage, L. D., Roth, T. C., Cerjanic, A. M., Sinervo, B. & Pravosudov, V. V. Spatial memory: Are lizards really deficient?. Biol. Lett. 8, 939–941 (2012).CAS
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
12.Milton, K. Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to primate mental development. Am. Anthropol. 83, 534–548 (1981).Article
Google Scholar
13.Thornton, A. & Boogert, N. J. Animal cognition: The benefits of remembering. Curr. Biol. 29, R324–R327 (2019).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
14.Pravosudov, V. V. & Clayton, N. S. A test of the adaptive specialization hypothesis: Population differences in caching, memory, and the hippocampus in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Behav. Neurosci. 116, 515–522 (2002).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
15.Morand-Ferron, J., Hermer, E., Jones, T. B. & Thompson, M. J. Environmental variability, the value of information, and learning in winter residents. Anim. Behav. 147, 137–145 (2019).Article
Google Scholar
16.Hermer, E., Cauchoix, M., Chaine, A. S. & Morand-Ferron, J. Elevation-related difference in serial reversal learning ability in a nonscatter hoarding passerine. Behav. Ecol. 29, 840–847 (2018).Article
Google Scholar
17.Boyle, A. W., Sandercock, B. K. & Martin, K. Patterns and drivers of intraspecific variation in avian life history along elevational gradients: A meta-analysis. Biol. Rev. 91, 469–482 (2016).Article
Google Scholar
18.Roth, T. C. II. & Pravosudov, V. V. Hippocampal volumes and neuron numbers increase along a gradient of environmental harshness: A large-scale comparison. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 401–405 (2009).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
19.Körner, C. The use of ‘altitude’ in ecological research. Trends Ecol. Evol. 22, 569–574 (2007).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
20.Roth, T. C. II., LaDage, L. D. & Pravosudov, V. V. Learning capabilities enhanced in harsh environments: A common garden approach. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 3187–3193 (2010).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
21.Tello-Ramos, M. C., Branch, C. L., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Pitera, A. M. & Pravosudov, V. V. Spatial memory and cognitive flexibility trade-offs: to be or not to be flexible, that is the question. Anim. Behav. 1, 1–8 (2018).
Google Scholar
22.Gonzalez, R. C., Behrend, E. R. & Bitterman, M. E. Reversal learning and forgetting in bird and fish. Science 158, 519–521 (1967).CAS
PubMed
Article
ADS
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
23.Strang, C. G. & Sherry, D. F. Serial reversal learning in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens). Anim. Cogn. 17, 723–734 (2014).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
24.Herszage, J. & Censor, N. Modulation of learning and memory: A shared framework for interference and generalization. Neuroscience 392, 270–280 (2018).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
25.Squier, L. H. Reversal learning improvement in the fish Astronotus ocellatus (Oscar). Psychon. Sci. 14, 143–144 (1969).Article
Google Scholar
26.Miyashita, Y., Nakajima, S. & Imada, H. Differential outcome effect in the horse. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 74, 245–253 (2000).CAS
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
27.Missaire, M. et al. Long-term effects of interference on short-term memory performance in the rat. PLoS ONE 12, 1–18 (2017).Article
CAS
Google Scholar
28.Bublitz, A., Weinhold, S. R., Strobel, S., Dehnhardt, G. & Hanke, F. D. Reconsideration of serial visual reversal learning in octopus (Octopus vulgaris) from a methodological perspective. Front. Physiol. 8, 1–11 (2017).Article
Google Scholar
29.Chittka, L. Sensorimotor learning in bumblebees: Long-term retention and reversal training. J. Exp. Biol. 201, 515–524 (1998).Article
Google Scholar
30.Chrobak, J. J., Hinman, J. R. & Sabolek, H. R. Revealing past memories: Proactive interference and ketamine-induced memory deficits. J. Neurosci. 28, 4512–4520 (2008).CAS
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
31.Malleret, G. et al. Bidirectional regulation of hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity and its influence on opposing forms of memory. J. Neurosci. 30, 3813–3825 (2010).CAS
PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
32.Joseph, M. A. et al. Differential involvement of the dentate gyrus in adaptive forgetting in the rat. PLoS ONE 10, 1–17 (2015).
Google Scholar
33.Shiflett, M. W., Rankin, A. Z., Tomaszycki, M. L. & DeVoogd, T. J. Cannabinoid inhibition improves memory in food-storing birds, but with a cost. Proc. R. Soc. B. 271, 2043–2048 (2004).CAS
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
34.Meck, W. H. & Williams, C. L. Choline supplementation during prenatal development reduces proactive interference in spatial memory. Dev. Brain Res. 118, 51–59 (1999).CAS
Article
Google Scholar
35.Clayton, N. S. & Krebs, J. R. One-trial associative memory: Comparison of food-storing and nonstoring species of birds. Anim. Learn. Behav. 22, 366–372 (1994).Article
Google Scholar
36.McGregor, A. & Healy, S. D. Spatial accuracy in food-storing and nonstoring birds. Anim. Behav. 58, 727–734 (1999).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
37.Healy, S. D. Memory for objects and positions: Delayed non-matching-to-sample in storing and non-storing tits. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. B 48, 179–191 (1995).
Google Scholar
38.Healy, S. D. & Krebs, J. R. Delayed-matching-to-sample by marsh tits and great tits. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B 45, 33–47 (1992).CAS
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
39.Hampton, R. R., Shettleworth, S. J. & Westwood, R. P. Proactive interference, recency, and associative strength: Comparisons of black-capped chickadees and dark-eyed juncos. Anim. Learn. Behav. 26, 475–485 (1998).Article
Google Scholar
40.Tello-Ramos, M. C. et al. Memory in wild mountain chickadees from different elevations: Comparing first-year birds with older survivors. Anim. Behav. 137, 149–160 (2018).Article
Google Scholar
41.Croston, R. et al. Predictably harsh environment is associated with reduced cognitive flexibility in wild food-caching mountain chickadees. Anim. Behav. 123, 139–149 (2017).Article
Google Scholar
42.Careau, V. & Wilson, R. S. Of uberfleas and krakens: Detecting trade-offs using mixed models. Integr. Comp. Biol. 57, 362–371 (2017).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
43.Niemelä, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. On the usage of single measurements in behavioural ecology research on individual differences. Anim. Behav. 145, 99–105 (2018).Article
Google Scholar
44.Gosler, A. G. The Great Tit (Hamlyn, 1993).
Google Scholar
45.Lejeune, L. et al. Environmental effects on parental care visitation patterns in blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. Front. Ecol. Evol. 7, 1–15 (2019).Article
Google Scholar
46.Bründl, A. C. et al. Experimentally induced increases in fecundity lead to greater nestling care in blue tits. Proc. R. Soc. B. 286, 20191013 (2019).PubMed
Article
CAS
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
47.Thompson, M. J. & Morand-Ferron, J. Food caching in city birds: Urbanization and exploration do not predict spatial memory in scatter hoarders. Anim. Cogn. 22, 743–756 (2019).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
48.Roth, T. C. II., LaDage, L. D., Freas, C. A. & Pravosudov, V. V. Variation in memory and the hippocampus across populations from different climates: A common garden approach. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 402–410 (2012).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
49.Griffin, A. S., Guillette, L. M. & Healy, S. D. Cognition and personality: An analysis of an emerging field. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 207–214 (2015).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
50.Ashton, B. J., Thornton, A. & Ridley, A. R. An intraspecific appraisal of the social intelligence hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 373, 20170288 (2018).Article
Google Scholar
51.Croston, R., Branch, C. L., Kozlovsky, D. Y., Dukas, R. & Pravosudov, V. V. Heritability and the evolution of cognitive traits. Behav. Ecol. 26, 1447–1459 (2015).Article
Google Scholar
52.Bründl, A. C. et al. Elevational gradients as a model for understanding associations among temperature, breeding phenology and success. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8, 56377 (2020).Article
Google Scholar
53.Freas, C. A., LaDage, L. D., Roth, T. C. II. & Pravosudov, V. V. Elevation-related differences in memory and the hippocampus in mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli. Anim. Behav. 84, 121–127 (2012).Article
Google Scholar
54.Pravosudov, V. V. & Roth, T. C. II. Cognitive ecology of food hoarding: The evolution of spatial memory and the hippocampus. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 44, 173–193 (2013).Article
Google Scholar
55.Croston, R. et al. Potential mechanisms driving population variation in spatial memory and the hippocampus in food-caching chickadees. Integr. Comp. Biol. 55, 354–371 (2015).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
56.Kozlovsky, D. Y., Weissgerber, E. A. & Pravosudov, V. V. What makes specialized food-caching mountain chickadees successful city slickers?. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20162613 (2017).PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
57.Izquierdo, A., Brigman, J. L., Radke, A. K., Rudebeck, P. H. & Holmes, A. The neural basis of reversal learning: An updated perspective. Neuroscience 345, 12–26 (2017).CAS
PubMed
Article
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
58.Cauchoix, M. et al. The repeatability of cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience 373, 20170281 (2018).
Google Scholar
59.Croston, R. et al. Individual variation in spatial memory performance in wild mountain chickadees from different elevations. Anim. Behav. 111, 225–234 (2016).Article
Google Scholar
60.Svensson, L. Identification Guide to European Passerines (British Trust for Ornithology, 1992).
Google Scholar
61.Friard, O. & Gamba, M. BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330 (2016).Article
Google Scholar
62.Tillé, Y., Newman, J. A. & Healy, S. D. New tests for departures from random behavior in spatial memory experiments. Anim. Learn. Behav. 24, 327–340 (1996).Article
Google Scholar
63.Bates, D. et al. Linear Mixed-Effects using ‘Eigen’ and S4 1–113 (Springer, 2016).
Google Scholar
64.Kuznetsova, A. & Christensen, R. H. B. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26 (2017).Article
Google Scholar
65.R Core Team. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2020).66.Warton, D. I., Lyons, M., Stoklosa, J. & Ives, A. R. Three points to consider when choosing a LM or GLM test for count data. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 882–890 (2016).Article
Google Scholar
67.Wilson, A. J. How should we interpret estimates of individual repeatability?. Evol. Lett. 2, 4–8 (2018).PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
68.Stoffel, M. A., Nakagawa, S. & Schielzeth, H. rptR: repeatability estimation and variance decomposition by generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 8, 1639–1644 (2017).Article
Google Scholar
69.Hadfield, J. D. MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: The MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1–22 (2010).Article
Google Scholar
70.Houslay, T. M. & Wilson, A. J. Avoiding the misuse of BLUP in behavioural ecology. Behav. Ecol. 28, 948–952 (2017).PubMed
PubMed Central
Article
Google Scholar
71.Kilkenny, C., Browne, W. J., Cuthill, I. C., Emerson, M. & Altman, D. G. Improving bioscience research reporting: The arrive guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol. 8, 6–10 (2010).Article
CAS
Google Scholar More